PROOF COVER SHEET Author(s):

Genı´ s Cardona and Noa Quevedo

Article title: Blinking and Driving: the Influence of Saccades and Cognitive Workload Article no:

NCER_A_841256

Enclosures:

1) Query sheet 2) Article proofs

Dear Author, Please check these proofs carefully. It is the responsibility of the corresponding author to check against the original manuscript and approve or amend these proofs. A second proof is not normally provided. Informa Healthcare cannot be held responsible for uncorrected errors, even if introduced during the composition process. The journal reserves the right to charge for excessive author alterations, or for changes requested after the proofing stage has concluded. The following queries have arisen during the editing of your manuscript and are marked in the margins of the proofs. Unless advised otherwise, submit all corrections using the CATS online correction form. Once you have added all your corrections, please ensure you press the ‘‘Submit All Corrections’’ button.

Please review the table of contributors below and confirm that the first and last names are structured correctly and that the authors are listed in the correct order of contribution. Contrib. No.

Prefix

Given name(s)

Surname

1

Genı´ s

Cardona

2

Noa

Quevedo

AUTHOR QUERIES Q1:

‘‘Introduction’’ has been added as the heading for the text after ‘‘Abstract’’. Please confirm.

Q2:

Please provide department details for affiliation.

Suffix

1 2

Current Eye Research, Early Online, 1–6, 2013 ! Informa Healthcare USA, Inc. ISSN: 0271-3683 print / 1460-2202 online DOI: 10.3109/02713683.2013.841256

59 60

3

61

4

62

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12

64

Blinking and Driving: the Influence of Saccades and Cognitive Workload

65

Genı´s Cardona and Noa Quevedo

69

13 14 Q2 15

63

66 67 68 70 71

University Vision Centre Research Group, Terrassa School of Optics and Optometry, Technical University of Catalonia, Catalonia, Spain

16 17

72 73 74 75

18

76

19

ABSTRACT

77

20

Purpose: The aim of this study was to investigate the joint influence of cognitive demands and large amplitude saccades on spontaneous blink rate. Materials and Methods: Twenty healthy volunteers were enrolled in the study and instructed to follow a 60minute real-life driving circuit while a video camera-recorded ocular movements and blinking. Five different complexity levels were predefined in terms of driving difficulty and traffic intensity, that is, cognitive workload. Five one-minute segments were selected for each complexity level in each driving trial, whereupon spontaneous blink rate and horizontal saccades were monitored. Saccades were classified according to their amplitude and blink-saccade pairs were recorded. Results: Albeit showing a high inter-subject variability, spontaneous blink rate and saccades were consistent within the same complexity level. At different complexity levels, no statistically significant difference in spontaneous blink rate was encountered, with an average of 20.3  1.6 blinks/minute (mean  SD). The number of large amplitude gaze shifts increased with the level of complexity (F = 15.403; p50.001). An average of 87.5% of large amplitude saccades were accompanied by a blink, and this percentage was similar for all complexity levels, that is, the number of blink-saccade pairs increased at higher complexity levels (F = 20.597; p50.001). Conclusions: In a complex, dynamic visual setting, spontaneous blinking accompanying large amplitude saccades may help in counteracting the reduction in blink rate associated with high cognitive demands.

78

Keywords: Blinking, cognitive demands, driving, saccadic eye movements, spontaneous blink rate

95

21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37

79 80 81 82 83 84 85 86 87 88 89 90 91 92 93 94

38

96

39

97

40 Q141

concurrent visual task.4,5 In effect, previous authors, albeit noting a considerable inter-subject variability in the frequency of spontaneous blinking,6 have reported mean  SD blink rates of 7.9  3.3 blinks/ minute while reading, 14.5  3.3 blinks/minute in silent primary gaze and 21.5  6 blinks/minute during a conversation.4 In addition, a similar reduction in blinking frequency with increasing task complexity has been evidenced in relation to computer use, a field in which spontaneous blinking has been extensively studied in order to understand and alleviate the symptomatology associated with the traditionally known as ‘‘computer vision syndrome’’.7–11 Interestingly, from a different perspective,

INTRODUCTION

42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54

Blinking has been described to occur voluntarily, reflexively or spontaneously.1,2 While voluntary blinks are associated with emotional expressions such as winking or smiling, reflex blinks are evoked by external stimuli (auditory, flashes of light, mechanical stimulation of the ocular surface or surrounding structures, etc.), or by ocular dryness resulting from the destabilization and rupture of the tear film.3 Conversely, spontaneous blinks are mainly dependent on cognitive processes, that is, appear to be regulated by a central pacemaker highly sensitive to the attentional demands and cognitive workload of the

55 56 57 58

98 99 100 101 102 103 104 105 106 107 108 109 110 111 112 113

Received 15 March 2013; revised 10 July 2013; accepted 2 September 2013; published online 2 2 2 Correspondence: Genı´s Cardona, Terrassa School of Optics and Optometry, Violinista Vellsola`, 37 E08222 Terrassa, Catalonia, Spain. Tel: +34 93 739 8774. E-mail: [email protected]

1

114 115 116

2 117 118 119 120 121 122 123 124 125 126 127 128 129 130 131 132 133 134 135 136 137 138 139 140 141 142 143 144 145 146 147 148 149 150 151 152 153 154 155 156 157 158 159

G. Cardona & N. Quevedo

the increased blink rate observed in studies investigating behavioral indicators of fatigue and drowsiness has been related to a cessation of the attention-driven inhibitor of blinks.12 Given the continuous flow of information reaching the visual system, each blink may account for a loss of approximately 400 ms.13 Consequently, to minimize the loss of potential relevant information, spontaneous blinks have been found to occur during explicit or implicit attentional breaks.14,15 Indeed, while reading, for example, blinks tend to be timed to occur at the punctuation breaks present within the text.15 Similarly to blinks, saccade eye movements are also accompanied by a suppression of visual input, thus explaining the visual stability that persists during ocular movements, despite the corresponding displacement of the image on the retina.16,17 Recent studies have observed a trend in which saccadic suppression seemed stronger for small gaze shifts (such as in reading) but weaker with increasing saccade amplitude.18 Many saccades have been found to be accompanied by a blink,19 and, even with the eyes closed, neuromuscular contractions associated with blinking tend to occur in relation to large ocular movements.20 Furthermore, the likelihood of encountering a blinksaccade pair increases with gaze amplitude, particularly when ocular movements are synchronous with head movements.20,21 These findings have led some authors to speculate whether blink visual suppression may be involved in facilitating visual stability during large gaze shifts, which correspond to the point of weakest saccadic suppression.18 With the aim of further exploring the relationship between blinks and saccades, this study evaluated spontaneous blink rate (SBR) and blink-saccade synchronicity while participants drove a vehicle on rural and city roads in real traffic conditions of varying complexity. It was believed that this experimental configuration would be able to provide relevant new information regarding the joint contribution of both cognitive and attentional demands and saccades to blink parameters in a daily-life setting.

160 161 162

MATERIALS AND METHODS

163 164 165

Participants

166

Twenty young volunteers (age mean  SD of 37.65  12.31 years; 8 females) agreed to participate in this study. All participants were non-commercial driving license holders and current drivers. Inclusion criteria were binocular visual acuity of 1 (decimal) or better with habitual correction, stereopsis equal or better than 50 arc seconds (measured with the Titmus Wirt test) and normal color vision (evaluated with the Ishihara color test plates). Subjects with binocular

167 168 169 170 171 172 173 174

vision problems, amblyopia, oculomotor anomalies and eyelid position (such as ptosis) or movement abnormalities were excluded from the study. All participants provided written informed consent after the nature of the study was explained to them. The study was conducted in accord with the Declaration of Helsinki tenets of 1975 (as revised in Tokyo in 2004) and received the approval of an Institutional Review Board (Universitat Polite`cnica de Catalunya).

175 176 177 178 179 180 181 182 183 184 185 186

Procedure

187 188

Participants were instructed to drive in silence during 60 minutes through an open circuit composed of country and city roads of varying navigational difficulty and traffic intensity. The same predefined circuit was followed in all cases, with the driving sessions taking place at the same hour in the morning to ensure similar traffic intensity. Only sunny days with good visibility were considered suitable for the purposes of this study. Aiming at exploring the influence of the driving associated cognitive workload on blinks and saccades, five different driving complexity levels were defined in terms of navigational difficulty and traffic intensity (see Table 1). Next, 25 one-minute segments were selected within the 60-minute circuit so as to include five segments from each level of complexity. Each one-minute segment related to a particular event along the route (e.g. entering a roundabout). Small, normal daily variations in traffic intensity were not taken into consideration to redefine the complexity level allocated to a particular segment. Participants wore their habitual visual correction and were asked to refrain from drinking coffee or alcohol prior to the beginning of the driving trial, as well as to have a good night rest of a minimum of seven hours of sleep. All driving trials were conducted in absolute silence, without ambient music or radio, except for brief instructions to navigate the circuit. Participants were told that their driving performance would not be evaluated and that video recordings were used to assess conjunctival redness. The same car with manual transmission was employed in all cases. Participants were allowed time to get accustomed to the driving characteristics of the car before starting the trial. They were instructed to adjust the lateral and rear view mirrors to ensure optimal vision. Seat position and steering wheel height were also adjusted to provide the best ergonomically correct position for each driver. Two different video cameras, fixed to the dashboard of the car, were employed to record the scene, as observed through the windshield (Sony DCR-SR32, 8 MP), and the face of the drivers (Nikon Coolpix S500, 7.1 MP), respectively. Current Eye Research

189 190 191 192 193 194 195 196 197 198 199 200 201 202 203 204 205 206 207 208 209 210 211 212 213 214 215 216 217 218 219 220 221 222 223 224 225 226 227 228 229 230 231 232

Blinking, Saccades and Driving

3

TABLE 1 Driving complexity levels according to navigational difficulty and traffic intensity.

233 234

Complexity level

235

1 2 3 4 5

236 237 238 239 240 241

291

Driving conditions

Traffic intensity

Car stopped at a traffic light Single carriageway with one traffic lane per direction Single carriageway with four lanes Simple roundabout Roundabout interchange or busy city intersection

– Light-moderate Moderate Moderate-heavy Heavy

Kolmogorov–Smirnov test, which disclosed normal distributions in all cases (p40.05). Therefore, comparisons between the different complexity levels were performed with an analysis of variance (ANOVA) test for repeated measures and, whenever a main effect reached statistical significance, post-hoc pair-wise differences were explored with a Bonferroni analysis. A p value of 0.05 or less was considered to denote statistical significance throughout the study.

242 243 244 245 246 247 248 249 250 251

254 256 257

261

FIGURE 1 Video stream showing the face of the driver (to assess blinking and saccades) and the scene as observed through the windshield of the car (to determine driving complexity).

262 263 264 265 266 267 268 269 270 271 272 273 274 275 276 277 278 279 280 281 282

Image Analysis The Pinnacle Studio 15 HD (Pinnacle Systems, Mountain View, CA) was employed to analyze the video recordings of the trial drives. Firstly, the recordings from both cameras were synchronized with the aid of the acoustic signal which marked the start of each driving trial, and fused into a single video stream (Figure 1). Secondly, for each one-minute segment, a frame by frame visual examination of video recordings was conducted to identify blinks and saccades and to determine the corresponding SBR and saccade frequency, as well as the number of blinksaccade pairs occurring within that time segment. Saccades were classified into small (approximately 10 or less) and large (more than 10 ) amplitude gaze shifts, and head movements accompanying large amplitude saccades were also noted. Only horizontal or approximately horizontal saccades were taken into consideration.

283 284 285

Data Analysis

286 287 288 289 290

295 296 297 298 299 300 301 302 303 304 305 306 307 308

311 312

255

260

294

310

RESULTS

253

259

293

309

252

258

292

Statistical analysis of the data was performed with the IBM SPSS Statistics software 20.0 for Windows (IBM Corp, Armonk, NY). All the variables under study were analyzed for normality using the !

2013 Informa Healthcare USA, Inc.

Spontaneous blink rate and the probability of observing a blink-saccade pair were found to follow a highly consistent behavior, that is, no statistically significant differences were evidenced between the five measurements corresponding to the same complexity level (ANOVA test for repeated measures, p40.05). A summary of the average SBR and blink-saccade pairs per minute for each complexity level is presented in Table 2 and shown in Figure 2. Spontaneous blink rate was found to be similar with independence of the complexity of the driving conditions (F = 0.591; p = 0.670), with an average SBR while driving of 20.3 blinks/minute. The amplitude of saccades was found to increase at high complexity levels, with many saccades involving a movement of the head as well. Thus, a similar number of saccades was found at each complexity level (19.5  1.1; F = 1.955; p = 0.108); the number of large amplitude saccades per minute increased at higher complexity levels (F = 15.403; p50.001) (Table 2 and Figure 2). A post-hoc Bonferroni analysis revealed statistically significant differences between complexity levels 1 and 2 (p = 0.025), 3 (p = 0.009) and 5 (p = 0.013), as well as between 2 and 4 (p = 0.009) and 5 (p50.001), between 3 and 4 (p = 0.003) and 5 (p50.001), and between 4 and 5 (p = 0.034). A similar percentage of large amplitude saccades was associated with a head movement at all complexity levels (an average of 59.4% of large amplitude saccades occurred in synchronicity with a head movement). The probability of encountering a blink-saccade pair was revealed to be dependent on the level of complexity, with an increasing number of blink-saccade pairs per minute at high complexity levels (F = 20.597; p50.001). A post-hoc Bonferroni analysis of these findings revealed statistically significant differences

313 314 315 316 317 318 319 320 321 322 323 324 325 326 327 328 329 330 331 332 333 334 335 336 337 338 339 340 341 342 343 344 345 346 347 348

4 349 350

G. Cardona & N. Quevedo

TABLE 2 Blinks and saccade eye movements per minute at each complexity level, with indication of the number of large amplitude saccades, as well as blink-saccade, blink-large amplitude saccade and head movement-large amplitude saccade pairs.

351

1

354

2

3

408 409

Complexity level*

352 353

407

4

5

Differences between Mean  SD complexity levels

410 411 412

355

Blinks (per minute)

19.6  9.0 19.1  10.2 18.7  9.4 21.9  9.3 22.1  8.9

20.3  1.6

356 357

F = 0.591 p = 0.760

Saccades (per minute)

18.4  3.8 20.3  3.1

19.5  1.1

F = 1.955 p = 0.108

Blink-saccade pairs (per minute)

11.7  3.8

6.8  2.4

6.4  2.7

11.7  4.2 15.0  4.5

10.3  3.7

F = 20.597 p50.001

417

Large amplitude saccades (per minute)

7.0  3.3

4.0  2.1

3.7  2.1

7.3  3.5 10.2  3.8

6.4  2.7

F = 15.403 p50.001

419

Blink-large amplitude saccade pairs (per minute)

5.9  3.3

3.4  1.9

3.6  2.1

5.8  3.5

9.1  3.6

5.6  2.3

F = 11.705 p50.001

421

Large amplitude saccade-head movement pairs (per minute)

4.2  2.0

2.4  1.3

2.2  1.3

3.8  2.3

6.1  2.3

3.8  1.6

F = 13.992 p50.001

18.2  2.8 20.4  3.9 20.2  3.8

358 359 360 361 362 363 364 365 366 367 368 369

Statistically significant intergroup differences are denoted by a p value of 0.05 or less. All parameters are presented as mean  SD. *Complexity levels: 1: Car stopped at a traffic light; 2: Single carriageway with one traffic lane per direction; 3: Single carriageway with four lanes; 4: Simple roundabout; 5: Roundabout interchange or busy city intersection

370

374 375 376 377 378 379 380 381 382 383 384 385 386

390 391 392 393 394 395 396 397 398 399 400

FIGURE 2 Frequency (per minute) of blinks, saccades, blinksaccade pairs and large amplitude saccades.

between complexity levels 1 and 2 (p50.001) and 3 (p50.001), between 2 and 4 (p50.001) and 5 (p50.001), as well as between 3 and 4 (p50.001) and 5 (p50.001), and between 4 and 5 (p = 0.046). Finally, Table 2 also displays the number blinks which occurred in synchronicity with a large amplitude saccade. A statistically significant difference was encountered between complexity levels in the number of blinks associated with large gaze shifts (F = 11.705; p50.001). An average of 87.5% of large amplitude saccades were accompanied by a blink.

401 402 403 404 405 406

416 418 420 422 423 424 425 426 427 429

372 373

388 389

414 415

428

371

387

413

DISCUSSION This study aimed at further investigating the welldocumented blink-saccade relationship by assessing

SBR, as well as blink-saccade synchronicity, in a reallife driving exercise in which different levels of complexity were defined according to navigation difficulties and traffic intensity. It was our hypothesis that high driving complexity levels, with the subsequent increment in cognitive workload, would be associated with a reduction in SBR, and with large gaze shifts, involving head movements. However, the effect of the apparently contradictory joint contribution of saccades and cognitive demand in determining actual SBR was unclear. In effect, large amplitude saccades and increasing cognitive workload have been documented to have an opposite influence over the frequency of spontaneous blinks. Thus, on the one hand, increasing cognitive demands have been reported to result in a reduction in SBR, both during computer tasks7–11 and other activities such as reading or watching a video story.4,5 On the other hand, blinks tend to be associated with large gaze shifts, rather than saccades of smaller amplitude such as those involved in reading,20,21 partially contributing to sustaining visual stability through momentary suppression of visual input,16,17 which is particularly relevant for large gaze shifts, were saccadic suppression is less effective.18 The present findings, although evidencing a moderate inter-subject variability, in agreement with previous studies,6 also revealed a high intra-subject consistency, with participants exhibiting similar blinking and saccade patterns within the same complexity level. Overall, driving was associated with an average SBR of 20.3  1.6. This value may be comparable with that reported by previous authors when examining SBR during tasks of medium cognitive workload such as during a conversation.4 Interestingly, however, SBR Current Eye Research

430 431 432 433 434 435 436 437 438 439 440 441 442 443 444 445 446 447 448 449 450 451 452 453 454 455 456 457 458 459 460 461 462 463 464

Blinking, Saccades and Driving 465 466 467 468 469 470 471 472 473 474 475 476 477 478 479 480 481 482 483 484 485 486 487 488 489 490 491 492 493 494 495 496 497 498 499 500 501 502 503 504 505 506 507 508 509 510 511 512 513 514 515 516 517 518 519 520 521 522

was not found to decrease at higher levels of driving complexity, remaining fairly constant throughout the whole driving trial. To advance an explanation for these findings, it is necessary to refer to our results regarding saccades and the probability of encountering a blink-saccade pair at each complexity level. The present experimental configuration only allowed for an overall and rather crude classification of saccades in terms of their amplitude. Thus, we differentiated between small and large gaze shifts and we noted the percentage of saccades also involving a head movement. Besides, only horizontal (or approximately horizontal) saccades were taken into consideration, although a preliminary examination of the video recordings showed a limited number of vertical saccades (fixation jumps between the scenery and the instrumentation panel of the vehicle), which was similar at all complexity levels. Similarly, equipment limitations prevented the recording of very small amplitude saccades and, although particular care was employed in the frame-by-frame examination of all video recordings, it may be assumed that an undetermined number of small amplitude saccades could not be identified, thus resulting in an actual underestimation of saccades and an overestimation of blinksaccade pairs. As was to be expected, high levels of driving complexity and traffic intensity resulted in a statistically significant increase in the predominance of large amplitude saccades, many involving a movement of the head (mainly with the purpose of checking the lateral rear view mirrors). In addition, the majority of large saccades were accompanied by a blink, as opposed to the small amplitude eye movements found at the lowest levels of complexity, many of which occurred without an associated blink. These findings are in agreement with previous publications20–22 and may give support to the hypothesis that blinking, when associated with large gaze shifts, may help in reorganizing attentional resources by momentarily suppressing attention and transferring it to a different region of the visual stimulus. Recent psychophysics studies investigating perceptual switching when viewing a Necker cube23 reach a similar conclusion. Therefore, it could be assumed that the reduction in SBR arising from a higher cognitive workload was counteracted, within the same level of complexity, by the increasing occurrence of blinksaccade pairs (gaze-evoked blinks), that is, the presence of large saccades may help in blocking the inhibitory mechanism which inherently decreases SBR during tasks involving high cognitive demands. It may be noted that at the lowest level of complexity, that is, when the car was stopped at a traffic light, participants tended to perform a number of large amplitude gaze shifts to scan the scenery while waiting for the light to turn green. Many of these saccades were also accompanied by a blink and, !

2013 Informa Healthcare USA, Inc.

5

therefore, given the low cognitive demand at this level of complexity, it probably should have resulted in an overall larger SBR than at any other complexity level. However, our findings failed to reflect this increase in SBR. Although the reasons behind these results are uncertain and warrant further investigation, it may be speculated that the cognitive demand of drivers, when waiting at a traffic light, was actually not at its lowest. A good addition to this study would have been to investigate the exact timing of each blink and saccade in terms of the specific information value of the scene observed through the windshield. However, this exploration would require more sophisticated recording and eye tracking instrumentation, not available to us at the moment of the study. It was therefore not possible to determine whether blinks while driving also tend to occur during implicit or explicit attentional breaks, as previously noted.14,15 In summary, the present findings give support to recent research describing spontaneous blinking as a complex mechanism regulated by a ‘‘central pacemaker’’ which, in turn, is modulated by, among other factors, the cognitive and attentional demands of the visual task at hand and the onset of large amplitude gaze shifts. Our results suggest that, when high cognitive demands are accompanied by an increase in the number of large amplitude saccades, the contribution of the latter is more significant in determining spontaneous blink rate. Therefore, the evaluation of SBR while driving may be considered a useful measure of the joint contribution of environment complexity and cognitive workload, opening possible new avenues of research in driving and blinking.

523 524 525 526 527 528 529 530 531 532 533 534 535 536 537 538 539 540 541 542 543 544 545 546 547 548 549 550 551 552 553 554 555 556 557 558 559 560

DECLARATION OF INTERESTS The authors report no conflicts of interest. The authors alone are responsible for the content and writing of the paper. No financial support was received in support of the study.

561 562 563 564 565 566 567 568

REFERENCES 1. Gruart A, Bla´zquez P, Delgado-Garcı´a JM. Kinematics of spontaneous, reflex, and conditioned eyelid movements in the alert cat. J Neurophysiol 1995;74:226–248. 2. Manning KA, Evinger C. Different forms of blinks and their two-stage control. Exp Brain Res 1986;64:579–588. 3. Delgado-Garcı´a JM, Gruart A, Trigo JA. Physiology of the eyelid motor system. Ann NY Acad Sci 2003;1004:1–9. 4. Doughty MJ. Consideration of three types of spontaneous eyeblink activity in normal humans: during reading and video display terminal use, in primary gaze, and while in conversation. Optom Vis Sci 2001;78:712–715.

569 570 571 572 573 574 575 576 577 578 579 580

6 581 582 583 584 585 586 587 588 589 590 591 592 593 594 595 596 597 598 599 600 601 602 603 604 605 606

G. Cardona & N. Quevedo

5. Veltman JA, Gaillard AW. Physiological workload reactions to increasing levels of task difficulty. Ergonomics 1998;41:656–669. ˚ kerstedt T, Peters B, Anund A, Kecklund G. 6. Ingre M, A Subjective sleepiness, simulated driving performance and blink duration: examining individual differences. J Sleep Res 2006;15:47–53. 7. Freudenthaler N, Neuf H, Kadner G, Schlote T. Characteristics of spontaneous eyeblink activity during video display terminal use in healthy volunteers. Graefe’s Arch Clin Exp Ophthalmol 2003;241:914–920. 8. Skotte JH, Nøjgaard JK, Jørgensen LV, Christensen KB, Sjøgaard G. Eye blink frequency during different computer tasks quantified by electrooculography. Eur J Appl Physiol 2007;99:113–119. 9. Wolkoff P, Nøjgaard JK, Troiano P, Piccoli B. Eye complaints in the office environment: precorneal tear film integrity influenced by eye blinking efficiency. Occup Environ Med 2005;62:4–12. 10. Himebaugh NL, Begley CG, Bradley A, Wilkinson JA. Blinking and tear break-up during four visual tasks. Optom Vis Sci 2009;86:E106–E114. 11. Cardona G, Garcı´a C, Sere´s C, Vilaseca M, Gispets J. Blink rate, blink amplitude, and tear film integrity during dynamic visual display terminal tasks. Curr Eye Res 2011;36:190–197. 12. Schleicher R, Galley N, Briest S, Galley L. Blink and saccades as indicators of fatigue in sleepiness warnings: looking tired? Ergonomics 2008;51:982–1010. 13. VanderWerf F, Brassinga P, Reits D, Aramideh M, Ongerboer de Visser B. Eyelid movements: behavioral

14.

15. 16.

17. 18. 19.

20.

21.

22.

23.

studies of blinking in humans under different stimulus conditions. J Neurophysiol 2003;89:2784–2796. Nakano T, Yamamoto Y, Kitajo K, Takahashi T, Kitazawa S. Synchronization of spontaneous eyeblinks while viewing video stories. Proc R Soc B 2009;276:3635–3644. Hall A. The origin and purposes of blinking. Br J Ophthalmol 1945;29:445–467. Higgins JS, Irwin DE, Wang RF, Thomas LE. Visual direction constancy across eyeblinks. Atten Percept Psychophys 2009;71:1607–1617. Wurtz RH. Neuronal mechanisms of visual stability. Vision Res 2008;48:2070–2089. Gandhi NJ. Interaction between gaze-evoked blinks and gaze shifts in monkeys. Exp Brain Res 2012;216:321–339. Fogarty C, Stern JA. Eye movements and blinks: their relationship to higher cognitive processes. Int J Psychophysiol 1989;8:35–42. Evinger C, Manning KA, Pellegrini JJ, Basso MA, Powers AS, Sibony PA. Not looking while leaping: the linkage of blinking and saccadic gaze shifts. Exp Brain Res 1994;100: 337–344. Von Cranach M, Schmid R, Vogel MW. The relationship between gaze movement and eye blink under various conditions. Psychol Forsch 1969;33:68–78. Nakano T, Kato M, Morito Y, Itoi S, Kitazawa S. Blinkrelated momentary activation of the default mode network while viewing videos. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2013;110: 702–706. Nakatani H, Orlandi N. Precisely timed oculomotor and parietal EEG activity in perceptual switching. Cogn Neuordyn 2011;5:399–409.

639 640 641 642 643 644 645 646 647 648 649 650 651 652 653 654 655 656 657 658 659 660 661 662 663 664

607

665

608

666

609

667

610

668

611

669

612 613

670 671

614

672

615

673

616

674

617

675

618

676

619

677

620 621

678 679

622

680

623

681

624

682

625

683

626

684

627

685

628 629

686 687

630

688

631

689

632

690

633

691

634

692

635

693

636 637

694 695

638

696

Current Eye Research