Vision and hearing are essential for person. Experiencing Age-related Vision and Hearing Impairment: The Psychosocial Dimension

Clinical Review Experiencing Age-related Vision and Hearing Impairment: The Psychosocial Dimension Vera Heyl, PhD, and Hans-Werner Wahl Abstract Epi...
8 downloads 0 Views 2MB Size
Clinical Review

Experiencing Age-related Vision and Hearing Impairment: The Psychosocial Dimension Vera Heyl, PhD, and Hans-Werner Wahl Abstract

Epidemiology

• Objective: To summarize the current state of research regarding the experience of age-related vision and hearing impairment. • Methods: Review of the literature. • Results: Negative consequences of age-related vision and hearing impairment manifest in the domains of health and longevity, everyday competence, cognitive functioning, social functioning, and subjective well-being. However, while vision impairment strongly impacts everyday competence, the burden of hearing impairment can mainly be found in the social domain. Psychosocially framed intervention research has shown promising findings, but many studies rely on small samples or do not include a control condition. • Conclusions: Although more research is needed, it is clear that traditional rehabilitation programs targeting age-related vision and hearing impairments need a strong psychosocial component.

Vision and hearing impairment is highly prevalent in old age, yet prevalence rates reported in the literature are quite different, depending on the definition of vision and hearing impairment used. A widely used criterion for low vision is the one used by the World Health Organization, ie, visual acuity less than 20/60 and equal to or better than 20/400 in the better eye with best correction. A best corrected visual acuity of less than 20/400 in the better eye is used to define blindness [4]. A disabling hearing impairment is defined by an average hearing loss in decibel (dB HL) of at least 41 dB HL at 500, 1000, 2000, and 4000 Hz. [5]. Translated to everyday life, such hearing impairment mainly manifests in severe difficulties in understanding normal conversation. Besides differing definitions, different methods to assess vision and hearing impairment and heterogenous study populations make comparisons of prevalence rates difficult [6]. In particular, relying solely on self-report data to assess vision and hearing loss seems generally problematic. In addition, the strong focus in vision impairment assessment on visual acuity measures has limitations, as other indicators, such as contrast sensitivity or useful field of vision, may be more important for out-of-home mobility or driving [7]. A recent study on the prevalence of visual impairment (defined as best corrected visual acuity < 20⁄40) in 6 European countries found quite similar prevalence rates as reported for the US: Prevalence of visual impairment was 3% in those aged 65 to 74 years, 13% in those over 75 years and 33% in those over 85 years [8]. At first glance, vision loss seems to be more prevalent among older women than among older men, but this relationship is not sustained in multivariate analyses considering

V

ision and hearing are essential for person– environment interaction and both are subject to pronounced age-related changes. Ongoing demographic changes and increasing life expectancy is contributing to a significant increase in the number of very old individuals [1]. It is projected that by 2030 about 50% of older Americans may have some significant eye disease, ie, cataract, glaucoma, or age-related macular degeneration [2]. Presbycusis as the major cause of age-related hearing impairment is present in 40% of the American senior citizens [3]. In this narrative review, we review the epidemiological data on age-related vision and hearing impairment, research on its psychosocial impact, and intervention research aimed to improve coping processes and rehabilitative outcomes. We close with future recommendations directed both toward research and clinical practice. www.jcomjournal.com

From the University of Education (Dr. Heyl) and Heidelberg University (Dr. Wahl), Heidelberg, Germany. Vol. 21, No. 7 July 2014 JCOM 323

Vision and hearing impairment age, health, and social support variables [9]. Regarding hearing loss, Gopinath et al [10] found prevalence rates of 29% among men and 17% among women aged 60 to 69 years. Moreover, for every 10 years of age, the prevalence of hearing loss doubled. In their review of epidemiologic data on prevalence of age-related hearing impairment in Europe, Roth and coauthors [11] report that at the age of 70 years, about 30% of men and 20% of women were found to have a hearing loss of at least 30 dB HL, while at the age of 80 years about 55% of men and 45% of women were affected. Lin et al found that 63% of those 70 years and older had a hearing loss of more than 25 dB in the better ear [12]. According to a recent review by Schneider et al [6], prevalence of impairment in both vision and hearing in older age (dual sensory impairment) varies between 1.6% and 22.5% due to different sample characteristics (eg, size, age) and different definitions and assessments of vision and hearing impairment (see also [13]). However, there is good evidence that dual sensory impairment increases with age, and that it is more common among frailer subpopulations such as older individuals consulting care services [6]. Quality of Life Impact of Vision and Hearing Impairment

Health and Longevity There is inconsistent evidence that both age-related vision and hearing impairment are accompanied by heightened multimorbidity and an increased mortality rate. For example, while some older as well as more recent studies have found that visual and hearing declines over time predict death in very old age [14–16], other studies have detected no significant relationship after adjusting for confounders such as age, gender, and education [17,18]. Among the hearing impaired, only men seem to have a significant increase in mortality risk [15,19]. Dual sensory impairment appears to be more consistently and more strongly related to increased mortality than vision or hearing impairment alone [19–21].

Everyday Competence The term everyday competence includes both basic (eg, self-care behaviors) and instrumental (eg, using public transport) activities of daily living (ADL/IADL [22]). Age-related vision impairment has been found to be robustly associated with significantly lower everyday competence, because visual capacity is a critical prerequi324 JCOM July 2014 Vol. 21, No. 7

site for such behaviors [23,24]. Indeed, lowered everyday competence appeared as the best of a range of variables (including cognitive function and well-being–related measures) used to differentiate between visually impaired and visually unimpaired older adults [18]. Furthermore, vision impairment impacts cross-sectionally as well as longitudinally—more strongly on IADL as compared to ADL—because the execution of IADL is more complex and depends more strongly on environmental enhancing or hindering factors [25–27]. Hence, shrinkage in IADL competence reflects a kind of early behavioral marker of severe vision impairment, whereas significant ADL decrease only happens later in the process of chronic vision loss. In contrast to vision impairment, age-related hearing loss has been found not to have a major impact in particular on ADL/IADL [28]. However, as has been found elsewhere [29], hearing loss is associated with increased reliance on community and informal supports, suggesting that while IADL function may not deteriorate with hearing loss, the way it is conducted may change (ie, need for support to maintain participation). It should also be mentioned in this context that assessment strategies have been developed to better consider the specific life conditions of those with vision and hearing impairment. The best-known and frequently applied instruments in this context are the National Eye Institute Visual Function Questionnaire [30] and the Hearing Handicap Inventory for the Elderly [31].

Cognitive Functioning Previous research largely supports the notion that reduced vision and hearing function is accompanied by a decrease in cognitive performance in older adults. The work of Lindenberger and Baltes, based on the Berlin Aging Study (BASE)—but also including additional studies with a wider age-range—is central in supporting a strong connection among vision, hearing, balance, and cognitive functioning in later life. Lindenberger and Baltes [32] found that general intelligence correlated just as strongly with visual as with auditory ability. In a model conjoining age, sensory function, as well as intelligence, visual and auditory function predicted a large portion of interindividual differences in intelligence and indeed fully mediated the negative correlation between age and intelligence. This finding has meanwhile been replicated by a number of other research groups and may be regarded as rather robust [33,34]. In addition, Baltes and Lindenwww.jcomjournal.com

Clinical Review berger [35] observed that sensory measures were better predictors of intelligence than socio-structural variables such as education or social class. They also showed that the connection among sensory functioning and intelligence was much closer in older adults as compared to adults in early and middle adulthood [34]. No clear difference between the sensory modalities of vision and hearing has been identified regarding their relationship with cognitive performance. On the one hand, there is research supporting the view that both vision and hearing impairment are connected with cognitive decline [12,36,37], while some evidence also supports that the linkage may be stronger with vision [14]. On the other hand, there are also data not supporting a close connection between vision and hearing impairment and cognitive function [38]. Explanations for such inconsistencies may refer to a number of reasons, such as pronounced positive selectivity of samples (which may lead to underestimation of connections among sensory and cognitive function), the application of established cognitive tests not appropriate for sensory impaired older adults (which may lead to overestimation of connections among sensory and cognitive function), and the application of different cutoff scores for significant vision and hearing impairment (possibly, higher cut-offs may lead to higher, lower to lower connections). Longitudinal data using the latest in causal modeling data analysis support the view that the causal dynamics involved in sensory and intelligence change are complex and that each of these variables can drive change in the other across longer periods of later life [39]. Vision status also plays a role when it comes to the connection between cognitive function and everyday competence—a linkage that is generally challenged as people age and that may lead to endpoints such as dependence on others and transition to long-term care. Heyl et al [40] observed that the link between vision status and out-of-home leisure activities is mediated by cognitive status. In a more recent study, able to add to the understanding of such a mediation process, Heyl and Wahl [38,41] showed that the connection between cognitive function and everyday function is much closer in visually and hearing impaired older adults as compared with visually unimpaired older adults, which possibly means that both visually and hearing impaired elders rely more intensely on their cognitive resources. Causality dynamics may however also work in the opposite direction. As Rovner and colleagues [42,43] observed in a study with age-related macular degeneration patients over 64 www.jcomjournal.com

years of age covering a 3-year observation period and 2 measurement occasions, activity loss over time due to the visual loss led to cognitive decline happening between T1 and T2. This finding fits well with the more general finding in the cognitive aging literature that the exertion of social and leisure activities is important for maintaining cognitive functioning [44].

Social Functioning Social relations as well as social support have generally been found to be of key importance for older adults [45]. Reinhardt [46] found that visually impaired older adults nominated on average 5.4 persons of intimate relation within their family network, and 3.5 persons within their friendship network, which is similar to sensory-unimpaired older adults, such as those assessed in the BASE [47]. In addition, in Wahl et al’s study [18], visually and hearing impaired older adults nominated practically the same number of persons as being in the most intimate circle of their social network (4.70 versus 4.71); the respective number in a comparison group of visually unimpaired older adults amounted to 5.2 persons, which was not significantly different from both sensory impaired group means. Neither vision nor hearing impairment seem to affect the experience of loneliness dramatically [18,23,48], although some research did report an increased risk of loneliness in older adults with vision impairment [49]. It is clear however that hearing impairment more strongly than vision impairment negatively impacts social communication and carries a strong stigma for those affected [48,50]. The stigma particularly implies that hearing deficits and concomitant communication disturbances (eg, giving an answer that does not match the question) elicits the view of a cognitively impaired, if not demented older person. In some contrast, vision loss seems to raise rather strong helping impulses and feelings of compassion. The dark side of this tendency is that it seems a challenge to provide visually impaired older adults with the instrumental support needed while at the same time fostering remaining capabilities [51]. Overprotection may put constraints on the visually impaired older adults’ “true” functional capacity and thereby contribute to loss in competence over the longer run due to disuse [52].

Subjective Well Being–related Outcomes and Depression Visually impaired older adults have shown evidence of diminished well-being as compared with sensory unim-

Vol. 21, No. 7 July 2014 JCOM 325

Vision and hearing impairment paired older adults [53], although effect sizes were rather small in a respective meta-analysis [54]. Differences in well-being between hearing impaired and unimpaired older adults seem small or nonexistent in some studies [18,55], but considerable in others [56]. The latter study covering a 16-year observational period as well as other longitudinal work (eg, [57]) also support the notion that remaining ADLs and social engagement mediate the linkage between sensory loss and well-being and depression. The “well-being paradox” in old age, pointing to pronounced adaptive resources to maintain well-being in spite of adverse conditions [58], may also apply to sensory impaired older adults [57,59]. At the same time, it is critical to acknowledge that visually impaired older adults represent an at-risk population, in which the positive impact of human adaptation and the drawback of reaching the limits of psychological resilience go hand in hand. Affect balance (ratio of positive and negative affect) has been found to be more toward the negative pole in visually impaired older adults [60] and depression has consistently been found to be significantly increased in visually impaired older adults [61–63]. Rates roughly vary between 15% and 30% and are particularly high in age-related macular degeneration patients [61]. This is also important, because depressive symptoms may accelerate both cognitive decline and decline in everyday competence in age-related macular degeneration patients [42]. Perceived overprotection may also lead to negative consequences in terms of heightened depression and anxiety over time [64]. Regarding the impact of hearing loss on depression, findings are quite inconsistent. Some studies found evidence for a significant relationship between hearing impairment and depressive symptoms among older adults [65, 66], while others did not [67, 68]. Gopinath and coworkers [66] observed that hearing impaired individuals, particularly women, younger than 70 years of age and those who were infrequently using a hearing aid (less than one hour per day) were more likely to suffer from depressive symptoms. According to a population-based study among older Italians, hearing impairment might be more closely related to anxiety symptoms than to depression [69].

Dual Sensory Impairment Previous research supports the notion that the overall psychosocial situation of those with dual sensory impairment is even worse as compared to those with sole vision 326 JCOM July 2014 Vol. 21, No. 7

or hearing impairment. In particular, higher rates of ADL/IADL impairment, depression and lowered wellbeing have been found in older adults affected by dual sensory loss [6,13,18,56]. Also, dual sensory impairment has been found to be linked with cognitive decline crosssectionally [37] as well as longitudinally [36]. Improving Quality of Life in Sensory Impaired Older Adults The research summary provided in the previous section underscores that the experience of age-related visual and hearing impairment comes with pronounced challenges that deserve evidence-based professional support. In the following, we give an overview and evaluation of major work in the area of psychosocially framed intervention research targeting older adults with vision and hearing loss. By psychosocially framed interventions we mean studies containing programs that focused on psychosocial processes (eg, consultation on how to better cope with sensory impairment, educative components, problem solving strategies, coping with negative affect) and assessed psychosocial outcomes (eg, everyday functioning, depression, emotional stress experiences). Such interventions may have been integrated into regular rehabilitation programs or offered as a separate strategy in addition to classic rehabilitation. We also consider physical activity–related and overall “way of living” interventions such as tai chi and yoga. In doing so, our aim is to highlight the bandwidth of psychosocial interventions and respective outcomes, not comprehensiveness.

Age-related Vision Impairment In a previous review paper, Wahl, Heyl, and Langer [70] analyzed 15 intervention studies including a considerable portion of randomized controlled intervention trials. These studies and some more recent intervention studies in the field of age-related visual impairment are listed in Table 1. Most psychosocially framed interventions could be characterized as self-management– and disease management–like efforts and are promising for visually impaired older adults. Major elements of such programs include stress-reducing strategies (eg, muscle-relaxation exercises), goal-directed problem-solving, strategies to evoke positive affect, activating available resources, and information and consultation. Typically, such programs are conducted in a group format in an eye clinic, bringing together 6 to 8 visually impaired older adults for weekly sessions of 2 www.jcomjournal.com

Clinical Review Table 1. Psychosocially Framed Intervention Studies Targeting Older Adults with Visual Impairment

Study

Design

Sample

Intervention

Primary Outcome Measures

Results

Effect Size

Self-management and problem-solving oriented, cognitive-behavioral and psycho-educative interventions Birk [101] Pretest-posttest 2004 Germany comparison group design (intervention vs. no treatment) Pilot study

Intervention: n = 14 Comparison: n = 8 Mean age: 73 years

Bradley [102] 2005 GB

Pretest-posttest single group design Pilot study

Intervention: n = 12 Mean age: 76 years

Brody [103] 1999 USA

Pretest-posttest randomized controlled trial (intervention vs. waiting list)

Brody [104] 2002 USA

Pretest-posttest randomized controlled trial (intervention vs. tape-recorded health lectures vs. waiting list)

Brody [105] 2005 USA

6-month follow-up randomized controlled trial (intervention vs. tape-recorded health lectures vs. waiting list)

Brody [106] 2006 USA

6-month follow-up randomized controlled trial (intervention vs. tape-recorded health lectures vs. waiting list)

Dahlin-Ivanoff [107] 2002 Sweden

4-month follow-up randomized controlled trial (intervention vs. standard individual program)

Eklund [108] 2004 Sweden

28-month follow-up randomized controlled trial (intervention vs. standard individual program)

Intervention: n = 44 Control: n = 48 Mean age: 79 years Intervention: n = 86 Control 1: n = 74 Control 2: n = 71 Mean age: 81 years Intervention: n = 82 Control 1: n = 66 Control 2: n = 66 Mean age: 81 years Intervention: n = 12 Control 1: n = 8 Control 2: n = 12 Only subjects depressed at baseline; Mean age: 82 years Intervention: n = 93 Control: n = 94 Median age: 79 years Intervention: n = 62 Control: n = 69 Mean age: 78 years

www.jcomjournal.com

Psychosocial intervention program with diverse components (eg, progressive muscle relaxation, problem-solving training); 5 group sessions over 5 weeks Peer group support and information; 6 group sessions over 6 weeks

Emotional wellbeing Behavioral functioning

Small to medium beneficial intervention effects in 5 out of 6 outcome measures.

Cohen’s d = 0.44– 1.14

QoL Well-being

Self-management program with diverse components (eg, information, problem-solving training); 6 group sessions over 6 weeks See Brody et al 1999 [103]

Emotional distress Self-efficacy

Most improvement in those reporting lower well-being at baseline; program was subjectively perceived as helpful. Reduced emotional distress and improved self-efficacy in the intervention group.

Emotional distress

Emotional distress declined most in the intervention group, especially for depressed participants.

See Brody et al 1999 [103]

Emotional distress

At the 6-month followup, emotional distress was still lowest in the intervention group; incidence of depression was lowest in the intervention group.

See Brody et al 1999 [103]

Depression

At the 6-month followup, greater reduction of depressive symptoms in the intervention group.

Health education program with diverse components (eg, information, skills training, modeling); 8 group sessions over 8 weeks See Dahlin-Ivanoff et al 2002 [107]

Perceived security in the performance of daily activities

The intervention group maintained or improved their level of perceived security, while those in the control group declined.



Perceived security in the performance of daily activities

At the 28-month followup, the intervention group still showed increased perceived security, while those in the control group showed further decline.





0.53 and 0.47 (for total scores)

Power of .80 to detect effect size of 0.50 (moderate effect size) Power of .80 to detect effect size of 0.50 (moderate effect size) Power of .60 to detect effect size of 0.85 (large effect size)

Vol. 21, No. 7 July 2014 JCOM 327

Vision and hearing impairment Table 1. Psychosocially Framed Intervention Studies Targeting Older Adults with Visual Impairment, continued

Study

Design

Sample

Intervention

Primary Outcome Measures

Effect Size

Results

Self-management and problem-solving oriented, cognitive-behavioral and psycho-educative interventions Eklund [109] 2006 Sweden

16-month follow-up randomized controlled trial (intervention vs. standard individual program)

Intervention: n = 43 Control: n = 49 Median age: 79 years

See Dahlin-Ivanoff et al 2002 [107]

Subjective experience of the program (content analysis)

Eklund [110] 2008 Sweden

28-month follow-up randomized controlled trial (intervention vs. standard individual program)

Intervention: n = 62 Control: n = 69 Mean age: 78 years

See Dahlin-Ivanoff et al 2002 [107]

Behavioral functioning

Kämmerer [111] 2006 Germany

Pretest-posttest comparison group design (emotionoriented vs. problem-solving-oriented intervention vs. no treatment) Pilot study

Short psychosocial intervention with emotionoriented vs. problemsolving-oriented focus; 3 group sessions over 3 weeks

Depression Problem-oriented coping Adaptation to vision loss

Rees [72] 2010 Australia

Single group design Pilot study

2- and 6-month follow-up randomized controlled trial (intervention vs. usual care)

Vision-specific self- management program; 8 group sessions over 8 weeks Problem-solving treatment; 6 individual sessions over 8 weeks

Participants’ perceptions of the program

Rovner [112] 2007 USA

Rovner [113] 2013 USA

3- and 6-month follow-up randomized controlled trial (intervention vs. supportive therapy)

Emotionoriented intervention: n = 23 Problemsolvingoriented intervention: n = 22 Comparison: n = 22 Mean age: 77 years Intervention: n = 15 Mean age: 76 years Intervention: n = 105 Control: n = 101 Mean age: 81 years Intervention: n = 121 Control: n = 120 Mean age: 83 years

Wahl [71] 2006 Germany

2-month follow-up comparison group design (emotionoriented vs. problemsolving-oriented intervention vs. no treatment)

Problem-solving therapy

EmotionSee Kämmerer et al 2006 oriented [111] intervention: n = 23 Problemsolving- oriented intervention: n = 22 Comparison: n = 22 Mean age: 77 years

328 JCOM July 2014 Vol. 21, No. 7

Depression Vision-dependent daily activities Valued activities

Vision function Vision-dependent daily activities Vision-related QoL

Depression Problem-oriented coping Adaptation to vision loss

At the 16-month follow— up, participants in the intervention group reported increased confidence to master daily tasks due to acquired skills, while participants in the control group referred mainly to the use of assistive devices. At the 28-month follow— up, the intervention group showed a higher ADL level, despite a comparable decrease in visual acuity in both groups. Tentative improvement R2 = in terms of depression 0.03–0.16 particularly in the emotionoriented group; increase in problem-oriented coping and adaptation to vision loss in the problemsolving-oriented group.

Program was subjectively perceived as useful in terms of emotional and behavioral outcomes. At the 2-month followup, positive effects were found in terms of depression and valued activities that diminished at the 6-month follow-up. At the 3- and 6-month follow-up, there were no group differences in terms of vision function, but group differences in vision-related QoL were in favor of intervention. Beneficial intervention effects disappeared from the pre- to the 2-month follow-up assessment.





Power to detect effect size of 0.40 (small effect size) See Kämmerer et al 2006 [111] for T1-T2

www.jcomjournal.com

Clinical Review Table 1. Psychosocially Framed Intervention Studies Targeting Older Adults with Visual Impairment, continued

Study

Design

Sample

Primary Outcome Measures

Intervention

Results

Effect Size

Physical activity and balance oriented interventions Chen [79] 2012 China

Pretest-posttest randomized controlled trial (intervention vs. music percussion activity) Pretest-posttest randomized controlled trial (intervention vs. standard program)

Intervention: n = 21 Control: n = 19 Mean age: — Intervention: n = 21 Control: n = 20 Mean age: 69 years

Miszko [80] 2004 USA

Pretest-posttest single group design Pilot study

Intervention: n = 8 Mean age: 54 years

Surakka [83] 2008 Finland

Single group design

Intervention: Physical training program; n = 24 (includ- group sessions 3 times ing 3 deafper week for 5–6 weeks blind participants) Mean age: 54 years

Kovács [77] 2012 Hungary

Modified 8-form Yang style Tai Chi; group sessions 3 times per week for 16 weeks

Balance control

Patient-tailored multi- modal exercise program in addition to standard osteoporosis exercise program; group sessions 4 times per week for 6 months Tai Chi exercise program in addition to O&M training; group sessions twice a week for 8 weeks

Functional balance Functional ability Functional independence

Muscle strength, work, and power of the knee Balance QoL Participant’s perceptions of intervention effects

Significant improvements in knee proprioception and in sensory organization compared with the control group. Significant betweengroup difference in functional mobility in favor of the intervention group.



Descriptively observed improvements in most of the variables.



All except 1 of the 24 participants reported improvement in at least one area (physical condition, mental state, balance).





Note: The column “Intervention” refers to the intervention of particular interest in the respective study. ADL = activities of daily living; O&M = orientation and mobility; QoL = Quality of life.

to 3 hours over 6 to 8 weeks. Group sessions are moderated by a clinical psychologist with professional experience with older adults. Positive effects have been reported in such self-management programs regarding depression, increased well-being, self-efficacy, and stress reduction. Dose of intervention is important; too-short and less-intensive programs may indeed elicit negative effects [71]. More recent work provides additional support for the usefulness of self-management programs for visually impaired older adults [72,73]. In addition, emerging evidence supports the notion that psychosocially framed interventions may contribute to saving health costs (eg, via reduced psychopharmacy) and may also enhance commitment toward secondary prevention, eg, avoiding later treatments of depression [74]. It is also obvious that such programs should find a strong liaison with classic high-caliber rehabilitation programs for visually impaired older adults, including effective reading training [75]. Furthermore, physical training programs, which have proven efficiency with old and very old individuals— www.jcomjournal.com

including those who are cognitively vulnerable—also seem to be of significant advantage for visually impaired older adults. As has been found, such programs not only increase posture, gait, and general physical fitness, they also prevent falls and enhance well-being, self-efficacy, and cognitive function, especially executive control [76]. Postural control has been improved by multimodal balance and strength exercises among older individuals with visual impairments as well [77]. Participation in physical activity and being in better physical condition buffered the relationship between dual sensory impairment and depression, pointing to the importance of physical training programs for the mental health of older persons with dual sensory impairment [78]. According to a randomized control study as well as to some case studies, tai chi seems to be an effective tool to improve balance control in visually impaired older adults, and thus to reduce an important risk factor for falls [79–81]. Visually impaired adults might also benefit from yoga in terms of balance improvement as well as psychosocial improvements [82]. To teach tai chi efficiently, it is necessary to adapt instruc-

Vol. 21, No. 7 July 2014 JCOM 329

Vision and hearing impairment tions to the needs of the visually impaired seniors by relying on verbal cuing and manual body placement [80]. The need to adapt instructions and to provide an accessible environment (including transportation arrangements) to motivate older individuals with visual impairments to perform regular physical exercises is also highlighted by Surakka and Kivela [83]. Furthermore, there is evidence that state of the art low vision rehabilitation as such also has beneficial effects on psychosocial outcomes, such as general and vision-related quality of life and emotional well-being.

Age-related Hearing Impairment Interventions concerning older adults with hearing impairment center on amplification and aural rehabilitation, including auditory training [84]. It has been shown that using hearing aids improves the quality of life, in particular hearing-related quality of life, of hearing impaired adults [85]. Yet many older adults who would benefit from hearing aids do not wear them [86]. From the reasons identified in the review by McCormack and Fortnum [86], perceived hearing aid value, in particular poor benefit in noisy situations, fit and comfort, as well as care and maintenance of the hearing aid emerged as most important. Improvements in these areas are necessary to enhance hearing aid usage among older adults with hearing impairment. Meyer and Hickson [87] identified 5 factors increasing the likelihood to seek help for hearing impairment and/or adopt hearing aids: (1) moderate to severe hearing impairment and perceived hearing-related everyday limitations; (2) older age; (3) poor subjective hearing; (4) perceiving more benefits than barriers to amplification; and (5) perceiving significant others as supportive of hearing rehabilitation. Thus, the involvement of family members in the rehabilitation process appears necessary and promising. Beyond amplification, aural rehabilitation seeks to improve the situation of hearing impaired older adults by providing listening and communication techniques to enhance communication effectiveness. We have summarized major work in this area in Table 2. Older studies have found empirical support for beneficial effects of conveying so-called hearing tactics such as lip-reading or asking for repetition to older adults in an individualized manner, based on a preceding functional analysis [88,89]. More recent studies likewise point to positive training consequences in terms of cognitive and social functioning and quality of life out330 JCOM July 2014 Vol. 21, No. 7

comes [90,91], particularly when significant others were involved [92,93]. But they also point to the fact that only a minority of older adults receives auditory and listening trainings and if so, compliance is low [94]. Tailoring auditory training to the specific needs and goals of individual clients, eg, training with single vs. multiple talkers, seems promising to increase compliance [94]. In sum, it seems clear for both vision impairment [95] and hearing impairment [96] that classic rehabilitation strategies, such as fitting a reading device or hearing aid, need significant enrichment by psychosocial training components in order to achieve the best outcomes possible. Furthermore, given the findings on the role of cognitive resources in visually impaired older adults (see respective section above), cognitive training may be an important addition to psychosocial intervention and rehabilitation [38,97]. It must be noted, however, that many of the available studies reveal a number of methodological limitations, such as small sample sizes, missing control condition, and no follow-up assessments to estimate the maintenance of effects. A significant future need is intervention research addressing older adults with dual sensory impairment. Although we found study protocols related to important trials underway [98,99] and a physical training study with visually impaired older adults that included also some dual sensory impaired individuals [83], it seems that there is not much research in terms of completed interventions and respective findings. Furthermore, it may be important to better involve significant others such as family members and friends in psychosocially framed programs and emerging research with low vision adults has revealed advantages and disadvantages of such an approach [100]. Practical Implications The older patient is on the way to become the “standard” patient for eye care and hearing specialists and thus a challenge for public health at large. Based on the evidence compiled above, we argue that best practice in medical treatment and traditional rehabilitation of vision and hearing impairment should better consider the psychosocial dimension of age-related vision and hearing impairment. We see different levels at which a stronger psychosocially framed input is needed. First, at the diagnostic level, having a better understanding of everyday competence, the role of cognitive functioning, social resources, and well-being–related dynamics www.jcomjournal.com

Clinical Review Table 2. Psychosocially Framed Intervention Studies Targeting Older Adults with Hearing Impairment

Study

Design

Primary Outcome Measures

Sample

Intervention

Results

Andersson [89] 15-month follow1994 Sweden up randomized controlled trial (intervention vs. no treatment)

Intervention: n = 10 Control: n = 10 Mean age: 69 years

Individualized behavioral counseling including the teaching of hearing tactics

Functional hearing ability Optimism Dealing with hearing loss

Andersson [88] Pretest-posttest ran1997 Sweden domized controlled trial (intervention vs. no treatment)

Intervention: n = 10 Control: n=9 Mean age: 72 years

Self-help hearing tactics program; 1 individual session and 4 telephone sessions over 4 weeks

Barcroft [114] 2011 USA

Pretest-posttest comparison group design (intervention 1 vs. intervention 2)

Meaning-based singletalker vs. multi-talker auditory training program; Individual sessions twice a week for 6 weeks

Burk [90] 2008 USA

Pretest-posttest single group design

Henderson Sabes [91] 2007 USA

4-week follow-up randomized crossover trial (intervention vs. delayed intervention)

Hickson [93] 2006 Australia

Single group design

Intervention 1 (singletalker): n = 34 Intervention 2 (multitalker): n = 35 Mean age: 66 years Intervention: n=8 Mean age: 70 years Intervention: n = 38 Intervention (4 weeks delayed): n = 27 Mean age: 65 years Intervention: n = 96 Mean age: 77 years

Communication strategies Dealing with hearing loss Behavioral obser- vation in difficult communication situations Auditory skills

Kramer [92] 2005 Netherlands

6-month follow-up randomized controlled trial (intervention vs. hearing aid fitting only)

Intervention: n = 24 (+ 24 significant others) Control: n = 24 (+ 22 significant others) Mean age: 70 years (significant others: 62 years)

Word-based auditory training program; 2–4 individual sessions per week for 12 weeks Listening and communication enhancement training program; 30 minutes per day, 5 days per week, for 4 weeks (10 hours total)

Word- recognition performance

Interactive communication education program; group sessions 2 hours per week for 5 weeks

Participants’ and significant others’ perceptions of the program

Hearing aid fitting plus self-administered home education program (dealing with communication strategies; speech reading) for hearing-impaired elders and their significant others; 5 sessions over 5–12 weeks

Emotional response Communication strategies

Speech perception Self-reported hearing handicap Communication strategies and attitudes

At the 15-month followup, the intervention group had improved in functional hearing ability and in dealing with hearing loss, while the control group had not. Beneficial intervention effects in terms of communication strategies, coping, and functioning in difficult communication situations.

Multi-talker auditory training group showed greater improvement on the multi-talker auditory skills test, whereas single-talker auditory training group showed greater improvement on the single-talker auditory skills test. Improvement in wordrecognition performance in noise. Those with the poorest scores on the baseline tests (eg, hearing handicap scores) were more likely to have greater improvement overall. However, variability was high among subjects. Overall, positive results were obtained. Satisfaction with the program and impact on others were particularly high At the 6-month followup, an improvement in communication strategies and quality of life was found in the training group, but not among the controls.

Effect Size —



η2p = 0.16 -0.74









Note: The column “Intervention” refers to the intervention of particular interest in the respective study.

www.jcomjournal.com

Vol. 21, No. 7 July 2014 JCOM 331

Vision and hearing impairment in visually impaired and hearing impaired older adults may significantly enrich the professional background knowledge about the patient. Such knowledge may become important for diagnostic evaluation, treatment decisions, and predictions of long-term outcomes. It seems also critical to have an understanding of the more fundamental mechanisms and systemic inter-relations in older patients (eg, among visual, hearing, mobility, and cognitive impairment), because such evidence helps to evaluate overall vulnerability and likely future trajectories of respective patients. Second, at the intervention level against the background of the available empirical effectiveness evidence, self-management–oriented and psycho-educative programs should become a regular component of low vision rehabilitation. Similarly, psychosocial programs educating older adults in hearing tactics and hearing loss– oriented coping strategies should become a regular part of hearing rehabilitation. In addition, we argue that cognitive training and physical activity–oriented interventions should have their place in rehabilitation programs designed for older visually and hearing impaired adults. The major reason is that respective programs generally have been found to positively impact quality of life in old age. This impact may be particularly valuable for more vulnerable populations, such as sensory impaired older adults. We therefore recommend implementing psychosocially framed programs as a regular service in eye clinics as well as in ear, nose, and throat clinics, because this seems to be the setting best suited to approach visually and hearing impaired older adults as well as to offer the logistic opportunities to conduct such programs. It would also be critical to extend such programs to in-home services as well as services covering long-term care settings. Older adults with dual sensory impairment bring specific challenges to such interventions, such as the optimal cooperation and combination of rehabilitation and psychosocial expertise related to each domain and traditionally offered side by side. It is also good news that new trials are underway to learn more about psychosocial interventions aimed to address older adults with dual sensory loss [98,99]. In conclusion, we argue for a better implementation of both age-related psycho-ophthalmology as well as psychoaudiology. Although we regard psychologists with a clinical training background as a key profession to be involved in psychosocially framed interventions with older adults with vision and hearing impairment, other professions (eg, 332 JCOM July 2014 Vol. 21, No. 7

occupational therapists, sport scientists) should also play an important role. A multiprofessional enrichment of classic sensory rehabilitation based on the training principles as described above is a major future need.

Corresponding author: Vera Heyl, PhD, Zeppelinstr, 1, D-69121, Heidelberg, Germany, [email protected]. Financial disclosures: None. References 1. Oeppen J, Vaupel JW. Broken limits to life expectancy. Science 2002;296:1029–31. 2. Eichenbaum JW. Geriatric vision loss due to cataracts, macular degeneration, and glaucoma. Mt Sinai J Med 2012;79:276–94. 3. Ciorba A, Bianchini C, Pelucchi S, et al. The impact of hearing loss on the quality of life of elderly adults. Clin Interven Aging 2012;7:159–63. 4. WHO - World Health Organization and International Agency for the Prevention of Blindness. Developing an action plan to prevent blindness at national, provincial and district levels. Vision 2020 The right to sight. Geneva: World Health Organization; 2004. 5. WHO - World Health Organizaton. Grades of hearing impairment. Geneva: World Health Organization; 2013. 6. Schneider JM, Gopinath B, McMahon CM, et al. Dual sensory impairment in older age. J Aging Health 2011;23:1309–24. 7. Clay OJ, Wadley VG, Edwards JD, et al. Cumulative metaanalysis of the relationship between useful field of view and driving performance in older adults: current and future implications. Optom Vis Sci 2005;82:724–31. 8. Seland JH, Vingerling JR, Augood CA, et al. Visual Impairment and quality of life in the Older European Population, the EUREYE study. Acta Ophthal 2011;89:608–13. 9. Horowitz A, Brennan M, Reinhardt JP. Prevalence and risk factors for self-reported visual impairment among middleaged and older adults. Research Aging 2005;27:307–26. 10. Gopinath B, Rochtchina E, Wang JJ, et al. Prevalence of agerelated hearing loss in older adults: Blue mountains study. Arch Intern Med 2009;169:415–6. 11. Roth TN, Hanebuth D, Probst R. Prevalence of age-related hearing loss in Europe: a review. Eur Arch Otorhinolaryngol 2011;268:1101–7. 12. Lin FR, Thorpe R, Gordon-Salant S, et al. Hearing loss prevalence and risk factors among older adults in the United States. J Gerontol Series A: Biolog Sci Med Sci 2011;66A:582–90. 13. Brennan M, Horowitz A, Su Y-p. Dual sensory loss and its impact on everyday competence. Gerontologist 2005;45:337–46. 14. Anstey KJ, Luszcz MA, Giles LC, et al. Demographic, health, cognitive, and sensory variables as predictors of mortality in very old adults. Psychol Aging 2001;16:3–11. 15. Appollonio I, Carabellese C, Magni E, et al. Sensory impairments and mortality in an elderly community population: A six-year follow-up study. Age Ageing 1995;24:30–6. 16. McCarty CA, Nanjan MB, Taylor HR. Vision impairment predicts 5 year mortality. Br J Ophthalmol 2001;85:322–6.

www.jcomjournal.com

Clinical Review 17. Ostbye T, Steenhuis R, Wolfson C, et al. Predictors of fiveyear mortality in older Canadians: the Canadian Study of Health and Aging. J Am Geriatr Soc 1999 47:1249–54. 18. Wahl H-W, Heyl V, Drapaniotis PM, et al. Severe vision and hearing impairment and successful aging: A multidimensional view. Gerontologist 2013;53:950–62. 19. Fisher D, Li C-M, Chiu MS, et al. Impairments in hearing and vision impact on mortality in older people: the AGESReykjavik Study. Age Ageing 2013. 20. Bamini G, Schneider J, McMahon CM, et al. Dual sensory impairment in older adults inceases the risk of mortality: A population-based study. PLoS ONE 2013;8:1–6. 21. Lam BL, Lee DJ, Gómez-Marín O, et al. Concurrent visual and hearing impairment and risk of mortality: The National Health Interview Survey. Arch Ophthalmol 2006;124:95–101. 22. Baltes MM, Maas I, Wilms H-U, et al. Everyday competence in old and very old age: Theoretical considerations and empirical findings. In: Baltes PB, Mayer K-U, editors. The Berlin Aging Study Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; 1999: 384–402. 23. Burmedi D, Becker S, Heyl V, et al. Behavioral consequences of age-related low vision: A narrative review. Vis Impair Res 2002;4:15–45. 24. Wahl H-W, Heyl V, Schilling O. Robustness of personality and affect relations under chronic conditions: the case of age-related vision and hearing impairment. J Gerontol B Psycholog Sci Soc Sci 2012;67:687–96. 25. Heyl V, Wahl H-W. On the long-term psychosocial adaptation to vision loss in the later years. In: Wahl H-W, Schulze H-E, editors. On the special needs of blind and low vision seniors: Research and practice concepts. Amsterdam: IOS-Press; 2001: 77–83. 26. Wahl H-W, Schilling O, Oswald F, et al. Psychosocial consequences of age-related visual impairment: Comparison with mobility-impaired older adults and long-term outcome. J Gerontol Psych Sci Soc Sci 1999;54B:P304–P16. 27. Wahl H-W, Becker S, Burmedi D, et al. The role of primary and secondary control in adaptation to age-related vision loss: A study of older adults with macular degeneration Psychol Aging 2004;19:235–9. 28. Rudberg MA, Furner SE, Dunn JE, et al. The relationship of visual and hearing impairments to disability: An analysis using the Longitudinal Study of Aging. J Gerontol Med Sci 1993;48:M261–M5. 29. Schneider J, Gopinath B, Karpa MJ, et al. Hearing loss impacts on the use of community and informal supports. Age Ageing 2010;39:458–64. 30. Mangione CM, Lee PP, Gutierrez PR, et al. Development of the 25-item National Eye Institute visual function questionnaire. Arch Ophthalmol 2001;119:1050–8. 31. Ventry I, Weinstein B. The hearing handicap inventory for the elderly: A new tool. Ear Hearing 1982;3:128–34. 32. Lindenberger U, Baltes PB. Sensory functioning and intelligence in old age: A strong connection. Psychol Aging 1994;9:339–55. 33. Clay OJ, Edwards JD, Ross LA, et al. Visual function and cognitive speed of processing mediate age-related decline www.jcomjournal.com

in memory span and fluid intelligence. J Aging Health 2009;21:547–66. 34. Salthouse TA, Hancock HE, Meinz EJ, et al. Interrelations of age, visual acuity, and cognitive functioning. J Gerontol Psychol Sci 1996;51B:P317–P30. 35. Baltes PB, Lindenberger U. Emergence of powerful connection between sensory and cognitive functions across the adult life span: A new window to the study of cognitive aging? Psychol Aging 1997;12:12–21. 36. Lin MY, Gutierrez PR, Stone KL, et al. Vision impairment and combined vision and hearing impairment predict cognitive and functional decline in older women. J Am Geriatr Soc 2004;52:1996–2002. 37. Tay T, Wang JJ, Kifley A, et al. Sensory and cognitive association in older persons: Findings from an older Australian population. Gerontology 2006;52:386–94. 38. Heyl V, Wahl H-W. Managing daily life with age-related sensory loss: Cogni-tive resources gain in importance. Psychol Aging 2012;27:510–21. 39. Lindenberger U, Ghisletta P. Cognitive and sensory declines in old age: Gauging the evidence for a common cause. Psychol Aging 2009;24:1-16. 40. Heyl V, Wahl H-W, Mollenkopf H. Visual capacity, outof-home activities and emotional well-being in old age: Basic relations and contextual variation. Soc Indicat Res 2005;74:159–89. 41. Heyl V, Wahl H-W. Cognitive ability as a resource for everyday functioning among older adults who are visually impaired. J Vis Impair Blind 2010;104:391–403. 42. Rovner BW, Casten RJ, Leiby BE. Variability in depressive symptoms predicts cognitive decline in age-related macular degeneration. Am J Geriatr Psych 2009;17:574–81 43. Rovner BW, Casten RJ, Leiby BE, et al. Activity loss is associated with cognitive decline in age-related macular degeneration. Thomas Jefferson University, Department of Psychiatry and Human Behavior, Faculty Papers. Paper 5. 2009. 44. Lövdén M, Ghisletta P, Lindenberger U. Social participation attenuates decline in perceptual speed in old and very old age. Psych Aging 2005;20:423–34. 45. Antonucci TC. Social relations. An examination of social networks, social support, and sense of control. In: Birren JE, Schaie KW, editors. Handbook of the psychology of aging. 5th ed. San Diego: Academic Press; 2001: 427–53. 46. Reinhardt JP. The importance of friendship and family support in adaptation to chronic vision impairment. J Gerontol Series B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 1996;51B:P268–P78. 47. Wagner M, Schütze Y, Lang FR. Social relationships in old age. In: Baltes PB, Mayer KU, editors. The Berlin Aging Study Aging from 70 to 100. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; 1999: 282–301. 48. Wahl H-W, Tesch-Römer C. Aging, sensory loss, and social functioning. In: Charness N, Park D, Sabel B, editors. Aging and communication: Opportunities and challenges of technology. New York: Springer; 2001: 108-26. 49. Nachtegaal J, Smit JH, Smits C, et al. The association between hearing status and psychosocial health before the age of 70 years: Results from an internet-based national survey on hear-

Vol. 21, No. 7 July 2014 JCOM 333

Vision and hearing impairment ing. Ear Hearing 2009;30:302-12 50. Wallhagen MI. The stigma of hearing loss. Gerontologist 2010;50:66–75. 51. Cimarolli VR, Boerner K. Social support and well-being in adults who are visually impaired. J Vis Impair Blind 2005 99:521–34. 52. Cicirelli VG. Fear of death in mid-old age. J Gerontol Series B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2006;61:P75–P81. 53. Horowitz A, Reinhardt JP. Depression among low vision elders. In: Stuen. C, Arditi A, Horowitz A, et al, editors. Vision rehabilitation assessment, intervention and outcomes. Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger Publishers; 2000: 655–8. 54. Pinquart M, Pfeiffer JP. Psychological well-being in visually impaired and unimpaired individuals: A meta-analysis. Br J Vis Impair 2011;29:27–45. 55. Tesch-Römer C. Schwerhörigkeit im Alter. Belastung, Bewältigung, Rehabilitation. Heidelberg: Median-Verlag; 2001. 56. Kiely KM, Anstey KJ, Luszcz MA. Dual sensory loss and depressive symptoms: the importance of hearing, daily functioning, and activity engagement. Front Hum Neurosci 2013;7:1–13. 57. Schilling OK, Wahl H-W, Horowitz A, et al. The adaptation dynamics of chronic functional impairment: What we can learn from older adults with vision loss. Psychol Aging 2011;26:203–13. 58. Kunzmann U, Little T, Smith J. Is age-related stability of subjective well-being a paradox? Cross-sectional and longitudinal evidence from the Berlin Aging Study. Psychol Aging 2000;15:511-26. 59. Schilling O, Wahl H-W. Modeling late life adaptation in affective well-being under a severe chronic health condition: The case of age-related macular degeneration. Psychol Aging. 2006;21:703–14. 60. Wahl H-W, Schilling O, Becker S, et al. A German research program on the psychosocial adaptation to age-related vision impairment: Recent findings based on a control theory approach. Europ Psychol 2003;8:168-77. 61. Casten R, Rovner B. Depression in age-related macular degeneration. J Visual Impair Blind 2008;102:591-9. 62. Crews JE, Campbell VA. Vision impairment and hearing loss among community-dwelling older Americans: implications for health and functioning. Am J Pub Health 2004;94:823–9. 63. Horowitz A, Reinhardt JP, Boerner K. The effect of rehabilitation on depression among visually disabled older adults. Aging Mental Health 2005;9:563–70. 64. Cimarolli VR. Perceived overprotection and distress in adults with visual impairment. Rehabil Psychol 2006;51:338-45. 65. Ishine M, Okumiya K, Matsubayashi K. A close association between hearing impairment and activities of daily living, depression, and quality of life in community-dwelling older people in Japan. J Am Geriatr Soc 2007;55:316-7. 66. Gopinath B, Wang JJ, Schneider J, et al. Depressive symptoms in older adults with hearing impairments: The Blue Mountains Study. J Am Geriatr Soc 2009;57:1306-8. 67. Chou K-L, Chi I. Combined effect of vision and hearing impairment on depression in elderly Chinese. Int J Geriatr Psych 2004;19:825-32.

334 JCOM July 2014 Vol. 21, No. 7

68. Tambs K. Moderate effects of hearing loss on mental health and subjective well-being: Results from the Nord-Trøndelag Hearing Loss Study. Psychosom Med 2004;66:776-82. 69. Bernabei V, Morini V, Moretti F, et al. Vision and hearing impairments are associated with depressive--anxiety syndrome in Italian elderly. Aging Ment Health 2011 15:467-74. 70. Wahl H-W, Heyl V, Langer N. Lebensqualität bei Seheinschränkung im Alter: Das Beispiel altersabhängige Makuladegeneration [Quality of life by limited vision in old age: the example of age-related macula degeneration]. Der Ophthalmologe 2008;105:735-43. 71. Wahl H-W, Kämmerer A, Holz F, et al. Psychosocial intervention for age-related macular degeneration: A pilot project. J Vis Impair Blind 2006;100:533-44. 72. Rees G, Keeffe JE, Hassell J, et al. A self-management program for low vision: Program overview and pilot evaluation. Disabil Rehab 2010;32:808-15. 73. Rovner BW, Casten RJ. Preventing late-life depression in age-related macular degeneration. Am J Geriatr Psych 2008;16:454-9 74. Eklund K, Sonn U, Nystedt P, et al. A cost-effectiveness analysis of a health education programme for elderly persons with age-related macular degeneration: A longitudinal study. Disabil Rehab 2005;27:1203-12. 75. Pijnacker J, Verstraten P, van Damme W, et al. Rehabilitation of reading in older individuals with macular degeneration: A review of effective training programs. Aging Neuropsych Cognition. 2011;18:708-32. 76. Colcombe S, Kramer AF. Fitness effects on the cognitive function of older adults: A meta-analytic study. Psychol Sci 2003;14:125-30. 77. Kovács É, Tóth K, Dénes L, et al. Effects of exercise programs on balance in older women with age-related visual problems: A pilot study. Arch Gerontol Geriatr 2012;55:446-52. 78. McDonnall MC. Physical status as a moderator of depressive symptoms among older adults with dual sensory loss. Rehab Psychol 2011;56:67-76. 79. Chen EW, Fu ASN, Chan KM, et al. The effects of Tai Chi on the balance control of elderly persons with visual impairment: a randomised clinical trial. Age Ageing 2012;41:254-9. 80. Miszko TA, Ramsey VK, Blasch BB. Tai Chi for people with visual impairments: a pilot study. J Vis Impair Blind 2004;98:5-13. 81. Ray C, Horvat M, Keen K, et al. Using Tai Chi as an exercise intervention for improving balance in adults with visual impairments: Two case studies. RE:view 2005;37:17-24. 82. Jeter PE, Dagnelie G, Khalsa SBS, et al. Yoga for persons with severe visual impairment: a feasibility study. Altern Med Stud 2012;2:18-25. 83. Surakka A, Kivela T. Motivating visually impaired and deafblind people to perform regular physical exercises. Br J Visual Impair 2008;26:255-68. 84. Li-Korotky H-S. Age-related hearing loss: Quality of care for quality of life. Gerontologist 2012;52:265-71. 85. Chisolm TH, Johnson CE, Danhauer JL, et al. A systematic review of health-related quality of life and hearing aids: Final report of the American Academy of Audiology Task Force on www.jcomjournal.com

Clinical Review the Health-Related Quality of Life Benefits of Amplification in Adults. J Am Acad Audiol 2007;18:151-83. 86. McCormack A, Fortnum H. Why do people fitted with hearing aids not wear them? Int J Audiol 2013 52:360-8. 87. Meyer C, Hickson L. What factors influence help-seeking for hearing impairment and hearing aid adoption in older adults? Int J Audiol 2012;51:66-74. 88. Andersson G, Green M, Melin L. Behavioural hearing tactics: a controlled trial of a short treatment programme. Behav Res Ther 1997;35:523-30. 89. Andersson G, Melin L, Scott B, et al. Behavioural counselling for subjects with acquired hearing loss. A new approach to hearing tactics. Scandinav Audiol 1994;23:249-56. 90. Burk MH, Humes LE. Effects of long-term training on aided speech-recognition performance in noise in older adults. J Speech Lang Hear Res 2008;51:759-71. 91. Henderson Sabes J, Sweetow RW. Variables predicting outcomes on listening and communication enhancement (LACETM) training. Int J Audiol 2007;46:374-83. 92. Kramer SE, Allessie GH, Dondorp AW, et al. A home education program for older adults with hearing impairment and their significant others: a randomized trial evaluating shortand long-term effects. Int J Audiol 2005;44:255-64. 93. Hickson L, Worrall L, Scarinci N. Measuring outcomes of a communication program for older people with hearing impairment using the International Outcome Inventory. Int J Audiol 2006;45:238-46. 94. Barcroft J, Sommers MS, Tye-Murray N, et al. Tailoring auditory training to patient needs with single and multiple talkers: Transfer-appropriate gains on a four-choice discrimination test. Int J Audiol 2011;50:802-8. 95. Wahl H-W. The psychological challenge of late-life vision impairment: Concepts, Findings, and practical implications. J Ophthalmol 2013. 96. Lin FR. Hearing loss in older adults. Who’s listening? JAMA 2012;307:1147-8. 97. Willis SL, Tennstedt SL, Marsiske M, et al. Long-term effects of cognitive training on everyday functional outcomes in older adults. JAMA 2006;296:2805-14. 98. Roets-Merken LM, Graff MJL, Zuidema SU, et al. Effectiveness of a self-management program for dual sensory impaired seniors in aged care settings: study protocol for a cluster randomized controlled trial. Trials 2013;14. 99. Vreeken HL, van Rens GHMB, Kramer SE, et al. Dual sensory loss: development of a dual sensory loss protocol and design of a randomized controlled trial. BMC Geriatr 2013;13. 100. Rees G, Saw C, Larizza M, et al. Should family and friends be involved in group-based rehabilitation programs for adults with low vision? Br J Visual Impair 2007;25:155-68. 101. Birk T, Hickl S, Wahl H-W, et al. Development and pilot evaluation of a psychosocial intervention program for pa-

tients with age-related macular degeneration. Gerontologist 2004;44:836-43. 102. Bradley P, Mitchell J, Bradley C, editors. Peer support for people newly diagnosed with macular degeneration: a pilot study. International Congress Vision 2005 September. London, UK. 103. Brody B, Williams R, Thomas R, et al. Age-related macular degeneration: A randomized clinical trial of a self-management intervention. Ann Behavi Med 1999;21:322-9. 104. Brody BL, Roch-Levecq A-C, Gamst AC, et al. Self-management of age-related macular degeneration and quality of life: a randomized controlled trial. Arch Ophthalmol 2002;120:1477-83. 105. Brody BL, Roch-Levecq A-C, Thomas RG, et al. Self-management of age-related macular degeneration at the 6-month follow-up. a randomized controlled trial. Arch Ophthalmol 2005;123:46-53. 106. Brody BL, Roch-Levecq A-C, Kaplan RM, et al. Age-related macular degeneration: self-management and reduction of depressive symptoms in a randomized, controlled study. J Am Geriatr Soc 2006;54:1557-62. 107. Dahlin-Ivanoff S, Sonn U, Svensson E. A health education program for elderly persons with visual impairments and perceived security in the performance of daily occupations: a randomized study. Am J Occup Thera 2002;56:322-30. 108. Eklund K, Sonn U, Dahlin-Ivanoff S. Long-term evaluation of a health education programme for elderly persons with visual impairment. A randomized study. Disabil Rehab 2004;26:401-9. 109. Eklund K, Dahlin-Ivanoff S. Health education for people with macular degeneration: Learning experiences and the effect on daily occupations. Can J Occup Ther 2006;73:272-80. 110. Eklund K, Sjöstrand J, Dahlin-Ivanoff S. A randomized controlled trial of a health-promotion programme and its effect on ADL dependence and self-reported health problems for the elderly visually impaired. Scand J Occupat Ther 2008;15:68-74. 111. Kämmerer A, Wahl H-W, Becker S, et al. Psychosoziale Unterstützung von älteren Menschen mit einer chronischen Sehbeeinträchtigung: Anwendung und Überprüfung einer problemlöse- und einer emotionsfokussierten Kurzintervention. Zeitschrift für Gesundheitspsychologie 2006;14:95-105. 112. Rovner BW, Casten RJ, Hegel MT, et al. Preventing depression in age-related macular degeneration. Arch Gen Psychiatr 2007;64:886-92. 113. Rovner BW, Casten RJ, Hegel MT, et al. Improving function in age-related macular degeneration: A randomized clinical trial. Ophthalmology 2013;120:1649-55. 114. Barcroft J, Mauzé E, Schroy C, et al. Improving the quality of auditory training by making tasks meaningful. Persp Audiol 2011;7:15-28.

Copyright 2014 by Turner White Communications Inc., Wayne, PA. All rights reserved.

www.jcomjournal.com

Vol. 21, No. 7 July 2014 JCOM 335

Suggest Documents