Assessing medication adherence self-efficacy among low-literacy patients: development of a pictographic visual analogue scale

HEALTH EDUCATION RESEARCH Theory & Practice Vol.20 no.1 2005 Pages 24–35 Advance Access publication 14 July 2004 Assessing medication adherence self...
Author: Rebecca Hicks
0 downloads 1 Views 184KB Size
HEALTH EDUCATION RESEARCH Theory & Practice

Vol.20 no.1 2005 Pages 24–35 Advance Access publication 14 July 2004

Assessing medication adherence self-efficacy among low-literacy patients: development of a pictographic visual analogue scale Seth C. Kalichman1,3, Demetria Cain1, Andrea Fuhrel1, Lisa Eaton1, Kari Di Fonzo2 and Thom Ertl2 Abstract

Introduction

Health behavior interventions are often grounded in Social Cognitive Theory, but instruments used to assess self-efficacy rely on verbal skills and yield scores that are highly positively skewed. Based on a review of the research literature and qualitative research with key informants, a pictographic medication adherence self-efficacy scale was developed. Two studies were conducted to test the pictographic and color visual analogue scale for assessing self-efficacy for medication adherence. Study 1 (N 5 81) demonstrated that the pictographic self-efficacy scale was internally consistent (a 5 0.68), time stable (2-week test–re-test r 5 0.63), and showed evidence for convergent and divergent construct validity. Study 2 (N 5 64) further supported the reliability of the pictographic self-efficacy scale with additional data supporting its convergent, divergent and criterion-related validity, including associations with medication adherence and HIV viral load. Distributions of self-efficacy scores approximated normal. This newly developed pictographic scale may be useful in assessing medication adherence self-efficacy in lowerliteracy populations.

People living with HIV-AIDS who are receiving antiretroviral medications must strictly adhere to multi-drug regimens to achieve optimal treatment responses (Barlett, 2002). Similar to other complex health behaviors, successful medication adherence is associated with an individual’s confidence in their ability to take their medications as directed (Gifford et al., 1996; Catz et al., 2000). Selfefficacy is defined as the personal belief that one can successfully perform a specific action under specified conditions (Bandura, 1997). In the case of medication adherence, self-efficacy beliefs correlate with self-reported and objectively measured missed medication doses (Eldred et al., 1998; Demas et al., 1998; Misener and Sowell, 1998; Catz et al., 2000). Although self-efficacy may be useful in understanding and predicting health behaviors (Bandura, 1997), self-efficacy is often poorly assessed. In a review of 65 published studies that assessed self-efficacy for HIV risk reduction, for example, Forsyth and Carey (Forsyth and Carey, 1998) determined that self-efficacy scales are often conceptually flawed and lack construct validity; 45% of studies that purportedly measured self-efficacy actually assessed constructs other than self-efficacy. Assessing self-efficacy in relation to specific situations requires construction of realistic and relevant scenarios. A limitation of self-efficacy scales that present individuals with scenarios of challenging situations, however, is that these instruments usually depend on reading comprehension and ability to follow the logical order of situations. Reading comprehension demands can be particularly

1

Department of Psychology, University of Connecticut, Storrs, CT 06269 and 2Center for AIDS Intervention Research, Medical College of Wisconsin, 8701 Watertoiwn Plant Road, Milwaukee WI 53202, USA 3 Correspondence to: S. C. Kalichman; E-mail: [email protected]

Health Education Research Vol.20 no.1, Ó Oxford University Press 2005; All rights reserved

doi:10.1093/her/cyg106

Medication adherence self-efficacy among low-literacy patients problematic in assessing HIV treatment adherence self-efficacy because HIV is most prevalent among populations with disadvantaged educations. To meet the need for a reliable and valid measure of HIV treatment self-efficacy for people with challenged reading skills, we developed a pictographic assessment of medication adherence self-efficacy. Pictographs have become commonplace in health education and health care [e.g. (Mayeux et al., 1996; Houts et al., 1998, 2001)]. Jacobson et al. (Jacobson et al., 1999), for example, demonstrated that a one-page educational handout with pictographs and minimal text increased pneumococcal vaccination more than five-fold over a control information handout. Similar findings were reported by Jolly et al. (Jolly et al., 1993, 1995). We built on pictographs used to improve the accessibility of health education materials to develop an assessment of adherence self-efficacy. In this research a color visual analogue scale was also developed to record self-efficacy responses. The visual analogue scale avoided relying on verbal anchors and provided a wide range of responses, reducing scale vulnerability to ceiling effects (Bandura, 1997). Visual analogue scales have been successfully developed for assessing a number of complex constructs including depression, anxiety, mood and pain (Cella and Perry, 1986; McCormack et al., 1988). Visual analogue scales offer psychometric advantages including sensitivity to change, semantic and pictographic rather than numerical anchoring, fine gradations between responses, and interval-level response data. We therefore performed a series of scale development activities which were followed by two studies that evaluated the psychometric properties of the new scale.

Scale development Previous research has identified barriers to medication adherence among lower literacy adults living with HIV-AIDS. For example, Kalichman et al. (Kalichman et al., 1999) found that 60% of people with HIV and lower reading literacy had missed doses of their medications because they overslept,

55% missed doses because they were busy and 30% missed their medications because unexpected things came up. Social situations also commonly delay taking HIV medications because of concerns about inadvertent HIV status disclosure (Catz et al., 2000). In addition, alcohol intoxication is a common barrier to medication adherence, with 17% of people who drink heavily missing their medications over a week’s time (Cook et al., 2001). We therefore selected three situations for assessing medication adherence self-efficacy: becoming unexpectedly busy in a social situation, oversleeping and alcohol consumption. To develop the scenarios for each of the three situations, we recruited 11 HIV-positive patients from area clinics to serve as key informants. These individuals were referred to the study by their case managers who were made aware of the opportunity to serve as key informants. HIV-positive men and women with less than 12 years of education, and who self-identified as having difficulty reading were asked about situations involving becoming unexpectedly busy, sleeping and alcohol use. Key informants were asked to generate stories from their own experience in which they had to remember to take their medications under the three conditions. We used participant descriptions of their own situations as the cornerstone for shaping the scenarios. For each situation, a graduated scenario was also developed to allow self-efficacy assessments under increased situational demands (Maibach and Murphy, 1995; Bandura, 1997; Forsyth and Carey, 1998). Key informants were asked to describe their experiences related to the situations as well as what would make the situations more difficult. In the final step, the three sets of scenarios (two scenarios in each set, total six scenarios) were then presented to the key informants to provide feedback on the scenarios and to help refine their content. A second group of three male and two female key informants discussed and rated all of the scenarios for their realism and personal relevance. Responses were tabulated and comments were integrated for revising the final generation of scenarios. After the final scenarios were established, each situation was illustrated in simple line-drawing 25

S. C. Kalichman et al. storyboards that coincided with a minimal situational description. A sketch artist was employed to generate the initial pictographic stimuli. We developed simple line drawings that depicted individuals who were of ambiguous ethnic backgrounds. In addition, we developed visual analogue scale response formats to remove verbal-dependent response categories and provide a wide range of responses. Table I presents the three sets of final stimulus scenes used for assessing medication adherence self-efficacy. The instructions for responding to the scenarios were read aloud, stating that each scene depicted a person living with HIV who is taking anti-HIV medications. Participants were told that the person has been instructed by their doctor to take their medications exactly as directed. Then participants were told that they should imagine being the person in the situation and respond how confident they were that they could take their medications exactly as directed in that situation. Each scenario was presented twice, with the second presentation increasing the difficulty level, as suggested by Bandura (Bandura, 1997). Increased difficulty of the scenarios was ascertained from key informants (see above). Following each scenario, participants were asked ‘How confident are you that you could take your

medications in that situation as directed?’ and marked their level of confidence on a color visual analogue scale (see Figure 1). Scores on the self-efficacy items were obtained by measuring the distance from left (Cannot do it at all) to right (Completely certain can do it) on each of the six visual analogue scales. Distance from the left edge to the respondent’s mark was measured in millimeters, yielding a range of possible scores between 0 and 255 mm for each response. Scores for the first gradation in each set were summed to obtain a subscore for the less challenging situation and scores for the second gradations were summed to provide a subscore for the more challenging situations (each subscore range 0–765 mm). A total score was obtained by summing the measurements across the six scenes (range 0–1530 mm) and divided by 6 to obtain a mean rating. The following two studies were conducted to examine the psychometric properties of the pictographic self-efficacy scale.

Study 1: Preliminary reliability and validity This study was conducted to provide an initial test of the reliability and construct validity of the

Table I. Scenarios presented with pictographs for assessing HIV treatment adherence self-efficacy Scene 1: Unexpected visit

Scene 2: Oversleeping

Scene 3: Alcohol use

You are coming home from shopping and run into an old (friend/lover). You haven’t seen him/her in a long time, so you decide to stop at his/her apartment for coffee. After a while you realize that you are having a good time talking to your (old friend/former lover). You do not have your evening medication dose with you and realize that you will have to go home right away in order to take the medication on time. You have had difficulty sleeping for the last several nights. Last night you tossed and turned but finally fell asleep at 3 a.m. Your alarm rings at 7:30 a.m. to wake you up, but you are still really tired. You don’t need to be anywhere today, but you remember the alarm was set to remind you to take your medication. The medicine is (sitting on your bedside table with a glass of water ready for you/in the cabinet in your bathroom and you have to get out of bed to get it). You are walking back from the bus stop and run into an old drinking buddy. He/She invites you to a bar for a drink. After a while, you have had (three/seven) beers and you’re having a good time talking with your old friend. (You are feeling quite intoxicated.) You do not have your evening medication dose with you and realize that you will have to go home right away in order to take your medications on time.

Language for gradations for increased task demands are shown in parentheses.

26

Medication adherence self-efficacy among low-literacy patients

Fig. 1. Color visual analogue format for medication self-efficacy responses.

pictographic medication adherence self-efficacy scale. The study focused on examining distribution characteristics, internal consistency and test–re-test reliability of the scale as well as evidence for convergent and divergent validity.

Methods Participants and procedures Participants were 52 men and 29 women living with HIV-AIDS recruited from clinical and community support services in Atlanta, GA. All participants were currently receiving antiretroviral medications. In terms of race, all participants were AfricanAmerican except for one man who was white. The mean number of years of education was 12. 3 (SD = 2.4), mean number of years that participants had been diagnosed HIV-positive was 10.9 (SD = 5.7), 23% of participants had CD4 cell counts under 200 and 46% had been hospitalized at least once for an HIV-related condition. All measures were administered in face-to-face interviews. Participants attended a second assessment session 2 weeks later to be reassessed on the pictographic self-efficacy scale. All of the study procedures were approved by the University Institutional Review Board (IRB).

Measures In addition to the pictographic self-efficacy scale, we administered a standard self-efficacy scale to assess convergent validity. To assess divergent validity we administered measures of self-efficacy for active engagement in medical care, self-efficacy for HIV disclosure decisions, self-efficacy for practicing safer sex and an AIDS-related knowledge test.

Participants also completed measures of demographic characteristics, health history and current medication adherence.

Demographic characteristics, health history and medication adherence Participants reported their gender, ethnicity, years of education and other common demographic characteristics. To assess medication adherence, participants reported their current medications and indicated whether they had missed any doses in the previous week. Responses were recorded Yes or No for having missed or not missed at least one dose of antiretroviral medications in the past week. Participants who missed at least one dose of their antiretrovirals were grouped for comparisons against those who did not miss a dose.

Standard medication adherence self-efficacy For convergent validity, we assessed HIV treatment adherence self-efficacy using items adapted from a scale reported by Gifford et al. (Gifford et al., 1996). This scale was selected because it represents the type of instrument most commonly used to assess self-efficacy (Bandura, 1997). The six standard self-efficacy items assessed confidence in one’s ability to overcome barriers to treatment adherence and maintain close adherence to medications. Example items include ‘I am certain that I can take my HIV medications exactly as my doctor tells me to’, ‘I am certain that I can take my HIV medications on time even if I have to wake up from a deep sleep’ and ‘I am certain that I can take my HIV medications even if I have been drinking 27

S. C. Kalichman et al.

Divergent self-efficacy Three self-efficacy scales were included to examine construct divergence from the pictographic treatment adherence self-efficacy scale. Three items were administered to assess self-efficacy for engaging in health care, e.g. ‘I am certain that I know when I need to call my doctor’ and ‘I am certain that I can understand what HIV is doing to my body’, a = 0.83. Self-efficacy for HIV disclosure decisions was assessed with four items, e.g. ‘I am certain that I can discuss being HIV-positive with a new sex partner’ and ‘I am certain that I can decide about disclosing my HIV to a new sex partner even if I have been drinking’, a = 0.87. Finally, self-efficacy for safer sex was assessed with five items that included ‘I am confident about suggesting using condoms with new sex partners’ and ‘I am confident that I can have safer sex and satisfy my partner’, a = 0.77. All items were responded to 1, Strongly disagree, to 4, Strongly agree.

AIDS-related knowledge AIDS knowledge was assessed using 19-items that included basic knowledge of HIV disease processes, transmission risks, medications and viral load (Carey and Schroder, 2002). All items were responded to Yes, No, or Don’t Know, with Don’t Know responses scored incorrect. We computed a score to reflect the number of items responded to correctly, a = 0.62.

Results Score distribution and reliability The mean score on the pictographic self-efficacy scale averaging across all six scenarios was 176.0 (SD = 38.0). The Skewness statistic was ÿ1.0 (SE = 0.26), indicating a slightly asymmetrical distribution with greater frequencies of lower self-efficacy scores. Kurtosis was 0.9 (SE = 0.51), indicating that the distribution tails were slightly longer than would be expected in a normal distribution (see 28

Figure 2). The total scale was internally consistent, a = 0.68. With respect to time stability, Time 1 to Time 2 correlations for individual self-efficacy items ranged from r(80) = 0.33, P < 0.01 for the first unexpected visit scenario to r(80) = 0.72, P < 0.01 for the second oversleeping scenario. The 2-week test–re-test correlation for the total scale was r(80) = 0.63, P < 0.01. The difference between Time 1 and Time 2 total scores was not significant, t(80) = 0.45, P > 0.1.

Differences between situation demands of the scenarios The correlations between less difficult gradations and the more difficult second gradations were significant, r(86) = 0.76, P < 0.01. To test whether the first gradations in each set resulted in higher self-efficacy ratings than the second gradations we compared within subjects mean responses. Results showed that the first gradations obtained significantly higher self-efficacy ratings (M = 573.6, SD = 116.5) than the second gradations (M = 480.5, SD = 128.2), t(85) = 10.0, P < 0.01.

Convergent and divergent validity Results from the correlations between the pictographic subscales for less difficult gradations, more difficult gradations and total scores with the standard medication adherence self-efficacy 30 25

Frequency

alcohol’. All items were responded to on scales ranging from 1, Strongly disagree, to 4, Strongly agree, a = 0.71.

20 15 10 5 0 50.00

100.00

150.00

200.00

250.00

Total Self-Efficacy Scale

Fig. 2. Distribution of scores from the pictographic medication adherence self-efficacy scale obtained in Study 1.

Medication adherence self-efficacy among low-literacy patients scale indicated reasonable correspondence between the two measures of medication adherence selfefficacy (see Table II). Importantly, correlations between the total pictographic self-efficacy scale and its subscales with the three divergent selfefficacy scales did not indicate any significant associations. In addition, the pictographic selfefficacy scale was not associated with AIDS-related knowledge.

Criterion-related validity Comparisons between persons who did not miss their antiretroviral medications in the past week (N = 61) and those who did miss their medications during that time (N = 20) showed that individuals who missed their medications obtained significantly lower self-efficacy scores for the two situation gradations subscales as well as for the total pictographic self-efficacy score (see Table III).

Study 2: Convergent, divergent and criterion-related validity The second study was conducted to replicate the reliability statistics observed in Study 1, and to perform additional analyses of construct and criterionrelated validity. In this study, we also focused on correlating the pictographic self-efficacy scores with potential confounding variables related to skills and abilities related to following the logic of the story or responding to the color bar scale, such as reading and numerical literacy, functional health, and neurocognitive indicators. Gender was also assessed as a potential confound because one set of scenarios was used for both men and women. We also examined the construct and criterion-related validity of the pictographic self-efficacy scale by performing correlations with indicators of medication adherence and by again comparing participants

Table II. Correlations among pictographic HIV treatment self-efficacy scores, standard scales of convergent and divergent self-efficacy, and AIDS knowledge (N = 81)

AIDS knowledge SE for safer sex SE for disclosure decisions SE for engaging in care Standard SE for adherence Less demanding scenes More demanding scenes

Pictographic self-efficacy scale

Standard and divergent self-efficacy scales

Total score

More demanding scenes

Less demanding scenes

Standard adherence

Engaging in care

Disclosure decisions

Safer sex

0.02 0.15 ÿ0.07 0.01 0.39a 0.90a 0.95a

0.06 0.21 ÿ0.01 0.09 0.38a 0.73a –

0.04 0.08 ÿ0.11 ÿ0.05 0.33a –

0.06 0.27b 0.19 0.23b –

0.23a 0.58a 0.24b –

0.20 0.28b –

0.11 –

a

P < 0.01, bP < 0.05.

Table III. Self-efficacy scores among persons who did not and who did miss their medications in the past week

Less demanding gradations More demanding graduations Total mean self-efficacy

Did not miss medications (n = 61)

Missed medications (n = 20)

M

SD

M

SD

596.9 505.5 184.2

92.2 120.4 32.9

487.3 386.9 145.7

151.3 116.9 40.8

t

3.9a 3.8a 4.3a

a

P < 0.01.

29

S. C. Kalichman et al. who had missed their medications to persons who had not missed their medications. This study was also IRB approved.

Methods Participants Participants were 43 men and 21 women living with HIV-AIDS recruited from AIDS clinical and community support services. The mean age was 42.9 (SD = 7.2, range 30–64). Eighty-nine percent (n = 55) of participants were African-American, 8% (n = 5) were white and 3% (n = 2) were of other ethnicities. The average years of completed education was 11.8 (SD = 1.9), with 32% (n = 20) of participants not completing high school; 83% were currently unemployed or on disability and 86% had annual incomes under $10 000.

Demographics and health status measures Participants completed the same demographic measures reported in Study 1. We also assessed whether participants were experiencing 20 different medication side-effects and whether they had experienced 14 specific symptoms of HIV infection. Participants also provided permission to contact their health care providers to access their most recent CD4 and viral load lab results to provide an objective index of their health status.

HIV treatment adherence measures Participants were interviewed to identify the HIV treatments they were currently taking and the doses taken in the past 3 days (Chesney et al., 2000). First, participants were asked to name the drugs they were taking. Next, trained interviewers asked participants to confirm the drugs they were taking by identifying their treatments among pictures of drugs shown on a chart. The interviewer proceeded to ask participants to think back about what they did yesterday and recall the times they had taken each drug. A daily calendar was used to help participants cue their memory and structure their responses. Interviewers recorded the number of doses reportedly taken yesterday for each drug repeated for the day before yesterday and the day before that. The number of pills missed in the past 3 days was 30

summed. In addition, participants were asked the number of pills missed in the past month and the time since the participant last missed taking their medications.

Potential confounds A number of measures were administered to examine potential confounding variables with pictographic self-efficacy scale responses. We assessed reading ability and reading comprehension using the Wide-Range Achievement Test [WRAT-3 (Wilkinson, 1993)] and the Test of Functional Health Literacy for Adults [(TOFHLA (Parker et al., 1995). The numerical literacy subscale of the TOFHLA was also administered (Baker et al., 1997). The physical limitations and physical energy subscales of the SF-36 (Ware et al., 1993) were scored as markers for functional health status. We also assessed depression using the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression (CESD) scale, a 20-item scale that assesses symptoms of depression over the previous 7 days (Radloff and Locke, 1986). Trails Test A and B (D’Elia et al., 1996) and the Clock Test (Schramm et al., 2002) were administered as measures of gross neurocognitive functioning.

Face validity To determine the perceived relevance and realism of the pictographic scale scenarios, participants were asked if they had ever been in a situation like the one depicted in the scene, whether the situation was realistic and understandable, and whether they could relate to the characters presented in the situation.

Results Comparisons of men (M = 154.6, SD = 42.2) and women (M =161.4, SD = 47.1) on total self-efficacy scores did not indicate significant differences, t(61) = 0.58, P > 0.1. Comparisons between men and women on the less demanding and more demanding gradation subscales also did not indicate significant differences. On average, the sample demonstrated low levels of reading literacy (missing 17% on the TOFHLA) and moderate levels of

Medication adherence self-efficacy among low-literacy patients depression (mean CESD score = 18.2, with a cut-off of 16 for moderate depression).

Differences between situation gradations As expected, the first scenarios and their second more demanding gradations were significantly correlated, r(63) = 0.74, P < 0.01. Comparisons of means on the less demanding gradations and more demanding gradations again showed that selfefficacy scores on the more demanding gradations (M = 141.4, SD = 49.0) were significantly lower than scores on the less demanding gradations (M = 171.6, SD = 44.1), t(62) = 7.1, P < 0.01.

Reliability The total pictographic self-efficacy score was internally consistent, a = 0.72. Table IV shows that the a coefficients if individual items were deleted indicated that no single item was inflating the internal consistency of the total scale. Also shown in Table IV are the inter-item correlations and corrected item-to-total correlations, indicating no signs of item redundancy and response dispersal. In addition, the Skewness and Kurtosis statistics indicated symmetric distributions (see Figure 3).

Tests for confounds Table V shows the correlations for the two selfefficacy subscales and the total score with tests of literacy, neuropsychological performance and functional health. None of the self-efficacy scores was associated with any of these potential confounds. Thus, self-efficacy ratings seemed unrelated to education, reading literacy, numerical literacy, medication side effects, HIV symptoms, physical condition, depression or neurocognitive functioning.

Face validity Participants were asked questions pertaining to their impressions of the scenarios and pictographs included in the adherence self-efficacy assessment (see Table VI). Nearly all participants indicated that the scenarios were realistic, that they could relate to the situations and that the scenarios were understandable. In addition, a majority of participants indicated that they had been in similar situations for each scene and there was variability in terms of whether participants had experienced difficulty taking their medications in similar situations for each scenario.

Table IV. Inter-item correlations, item distribution characteristics and a coefficients if items deleted

Go out with a friend for 7 drinks Go out with a friend for 3 drinks Over sleep with meds far Over sleep with meds near Run into an old lover Mean SD Skewness Kurtosis Item-to-total correlation a if deleted

Run into an old friend

Run into an old lover

Over sleep with meds near

Over sleep with meds far

0.35a 0.38a 0.20 0.15 0.76a 184.0 53.4 ÿ0.69 ÿ0.36 0.58 0.65

0.38a 0.48a 0.11 0.11

0.09 0.03 0.67a

0.24b 0.13

– 172.3 69.5 ÿ0.83 ÿ0.35 0.57 0.64

Go out with a friend for 3 drinks 0.48a

Go out with a friend for 7 drinks –





– 204.8 56.0 ÿ2.2 4.1 0.28 0.72

188.9 61.8 ÿ1.4 1.5 0.37 0.70

126.1 83.6 ÿ0.02 ÿ1.4 0.47 0.68

63.9 75.8 1.5 0.87 0.49 0.67

a

P < 0.01, bP < 0.05.

31

S. C. Kalichman et al.

Criterion-related validity

12

10

Frequency

8

6

4

2

0

50

70

90

110

130

150

170

190

210

230

250

Total Self-Efficacy Score

Fig. 3. Distribution of scores from the pictographic medication adherence self-efficacy scale obtained in Study 2.

Table V. Correlations among pictographic self-efficacy scores and potential confounding participant characteristics Less More Total demanding demanding score gradations gradations Literacy education WRAT TOFHLA-R (% correct) TOFHLA-N (% correct)

32

SD

ÿ0.09 ÿ0.14 ÿ0.16

ÿ0.02 ÿ0.20 ÿ0.12

ÿ0.15 ÿ0.07 ÿ0.17

11.8 1.9 40.9 11.8 83.3 18.4

ÿ0.12

ÿ0.07

ÿ0.14

76.7 16.2

0.11

0.03

11.2 10.2

0.06 0.22 ÿ0.02 0.10

ÿ0.10 0.16 ÿ0.02 0.12

14.5 13.5 6.3 5.2 22.8 5.6 18.2 12.2

Functional health medication 0.07 side-effects HIV symptoms ÿ0.03 SF-36 limits 0.15 SF-36 energy ÿ0.03 Depression 0.12 score Trails A 0.11 Trails B 0.15 Clock Test 0.18 Mean SD Skewness Kurtosis

M

156.5 43.4 ÿ0.02 0.01

0.07 0.14 0.20 171.6 44.1 ÿ0.31 ÿ0.09

0.13 76.4 43.2 0.13 134.1 94.9 0.14 7.7 1.8 141.44 49.0 0.36 ÿ0.11

Table VII shows the correlations among the two medication adherence self-efficacy subscales, and the total score with behavioral and biological markers of medication adherence. Results showed that self-efficacy scores were associated with missed medications, days since medications were missed, and chart abstracted viral load. All of the correlations were in the direction of higher self-efficacy being associated with better HIV treatment adherence. Comparisons of participants who did not miss their medications in the past 3 days with those who did miss their medications during that time period on the self-efficacy subscales and total scores showed that individuals who had missed their medications reported significantly lower self-efficacy responses on the less demanding situations and on the total score, with a trend toward significant difference for the more demanding gradations (see Table VIII).

Limitations Interpretation of the study findings requires attention to their methodological limitations. First, the studies presented here are limited by their reliance on self-reported medication adherence. Without an objective measure of medication adherence in addition to viral load it is possible that the observed correlations between medication adherence and self-efficacy are artifacts of response biases. Second, the studies had relatively small sample sizes, potentially limiting generalizability of the scale development analyses. Another methodological limitation is the discontinuity of time frames for the self-efficacy and adherence behaviors. Specifically, self-efficacy is assessed for the present, with individuals indicating how confident they currently feel. In contrast, all of the adherence measures referred to pills missed over a retrospective time frame. The data therefore do not allow for any causal inferences. The current studies also identified areas for further development of the pictographic medication

Medication adherence self-efficacy among low-literacy patients Table VI. Face validity of the three pictographic self-efficacy assessment scenarios

Been in this type of situation Experienced difficulty taking medications in a similar situation The scenario seemed realistic Scenario was easy to understand Able to relate to people in the situation The pictures helped to follow the scenario

Running into an old friend/lover

Over sleeping at medication time

Going out with a friend drinking

N

%

N

%

N

%

44 17

69 27

56 26

88 41

39 34

61 53

53 62 59 62

83 97 92 97

60 62 59 61

94 97 92 95

59 61 55 61

92 95 86 95

Table VII. Correlations among pictographic self-efficacy scores and indices of antiretroviral adherence and health status

Pills missed in past 3 days Pills missed in past month Days since last missed dose Viral load CD4 cell count

Less difficult first gradation

More difficult second gradation

Total score

ÿ0.25c

ÿ0.24c

ÿ0.22

ÿ0.28b

ÿ0.26b

ÿ0.26b

0.40a

0.34a

0.40a

ÿ0.32a ÿ0.11

ÿ0.34a ÿ0.13

ÿ0.32b ÿ0.07

a

P < 0.01, bP < 0.05, cP < 0.10.

Table VIII. Self-efficacy scores among persons with HIV who did not and who did miss their medications in the past 3 days

Less difficult gradation More difficult gradation Total self-efficacy a

P < 0.05.

t

Did not miss medications (n = 36)

Missed medications (n = 14 )

M

SD

M

SD

179.0

42.1

152.2

39.0

2.1a

146.5

48.0

120.1

34.3

1.9

162.8

41.8

136.2

34.1

2.2a

adherence self-efficacy scale. We found that the graded scenes for situation demands were not universally more difficult than the situations assumed less demanding. For example, some participants stated that it would be easier to leave and take their medications if they were visiting with a former lover than with an old friend. It may also be easier for some people to get out of bed to get their medications than to roll over and possibly fall back asleep before taking their medications at their bedside. Finally, although the drinking scenes clearly pose the greatest challenge to taking medications, the scenes may be less relevant for people who do not drink and many people do not drink while taking medications to avoid adverse reactions. Despite these limitations, we believe that the pictographic medication self-efficacy scale has implications for improving the assessment of selfefficacy in diverse populations.

General discussion Results of Studies 1 and 2 showed that the pictographic medication adherence self-efficacy scale developed in this research was internally consistent and possessed distributional characteristics that approximated normal. The self-efficacy scale also appeared face valid, with participants identifying with the scenes and recognizing the difficulties of adhering to medications within the situations. Results also provided evidence for the construct and criterion-related validity of the self-efficacy scale. 33

S. C. Kalichman et al. Pictographic self-efficacy scores were associated with behavioral measures of medication adherence and chart abstracted lab results for viral load— a biological marker for HIV treatment adherence. Importantly, the self-efficacy scale was not associated with potential confounding factors in medication adherence including reading and numerical literacy, education, and health status including functional health, depression, neurocognitive functioning, CD4 cell counts, HIV symptoms and medication side-effects. These findings provided evidence for the reliability and validity of the pictographic medication adherence self-efficacy scale. The pictographic self-efficacy scale was developed for use with persons who have limited reading abilities as well as other language challenges. However, we believe that the scale may have a more generalized use than those that we originally had in mind. The scale has internally valid situational gradations of task demands, normal distributions of scores and yields interval level data. The pictographic self-efficacy scale may therefore be adaptable to other chronic medical populations, particularly patient populations that require long-term medications, such as diabetes and asthma. Future research is needed to investigate the adaptability of the scale for various medical populations.

Acknowledgements This research was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health and the National Institute of Nursing Research R01MH/NR62287. The authors thank Moira Kalichman, Chauncey Cherry and the AIDS Survival Project of Atlanta for their assistance with this research.

References Bandura, A. (1986) Social Foundations of Thought and Action: A Social Cognitive Theory. Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ. Bandura, A. (1994) Social cognitive theory and exercise of control over HIV infection. In DiClemente, R. and Peterson, J.

34

(eds), Preventing AIDS: Theories, Methods and Behavioral Interventions. Plenum, New York, pp. 25–60. Bandura, A. (1997) Self-efficacy: The Exercise of Control. Freeman, New York. Bartlett, J. (2002) Addressing the challenges of adherence. Journal of AIDS, 29, S2–S10. Carey, M.P. and Schroder, K.E. (2002) Development and psychometric evaluation of the brief HIV knowledge questionnaire (HIV-KQ-18). AIDS Education and Prevention, 14, 174–184. Catz, S.L., Kelly, J.A., Bogart, L.M., Benotsch, E.G. and McAuliffe, T.A. (2000) Patterns, correlates and barriers to medication adherence among persons prescribed new treatments for HIV disease. Health Psychology, 19, 124–133. Cella, D. and Perry, S. (1986) Reliability and concurrent validity of three visual-analogue mood scales. Psychological Reports, 59, 827–833. Chesney, M.A., Ickovics, J.R., Chambers, D.B., Giffrord, A.L., Nedig, J., Zwickl, B. and Wu, A.W. (2000) Self reported adherence to antiretroviral medications among participants in HIV clinical trials: the AACTG adherence instruments. AIDS Care, 12, 255–266. Cook, R., Sereika, S., Hunt, S., Woodward, W., Erlen, J. and Conigliaro, J. (1999) Problem drinking and medication adherence among persons with HIV infection. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 16, 83–88. D’Elia, L., Satz, P., Uchiyama, C. and White, T. (1996) Color Trails Test. Psychological Assessment Resources, Lutz, FL. Demas, P., Schoenbaum, E.E., Hirky, E.A., Wills, T.A., Doll, L.S., Hartel, D.M. and Klein, R.S. (1998) The relationship of HIV treatment acceptance and adherence to psychosocial factors among injecting drug users. AIDS and Behavior, 2, 283–292. Eldred, L.J., Wu, A.W., Chaisson, R.E. and Moore, R.D. (1998) Adherence to antiretroviral and pneumocystis prophylaxis in HIV disease. Journal of Acquired Immune Deficiency Syndrome and Human Retrovirology, 18, 117–125. Forsyth, A.D. and Carey, M.P. (1998) Measuring self-efficacy in the context of HIV-risk-reduction: research challenges and recommendations. Health Psychology, 17, 559–568. Houts, P.S., Bachrach, R., Witmer, J.T., Tringali, C.A., Bucher, J.A. and Localio, R.A. (1998) Using pictographs to enhance recall of spoken medical instructions. Patient Education and Counseling, 35, 83–88. Houts, P.S., Witmer, J.T., Egeth, H.E., Loscalzo, M.J. and Zabora, J.R. (2001) Using pictographs to enhance recall of spoken medical instructions II. Patient Education and Counseling, 43, 231–242. Jacobson, T.A., Thomas, D.M., Morton, F.J., Offutt, G., Shevlin, J. and Ray, S. (1999) Use of a low-literacy patient education tool to enhance pneumococcal vaccination rates. Journal of American Medical Association, 282, 646–650. Jolly, B.T., Scott, J.L., Feied, C.F. and Sanford, S.M. (1993) Functional illiteracy among emergency department patients: A preliminary study. Annals of Emergency Medicine, 22, 573–578. Jolly, B.T., Scott, J.D. and Sanford, S.M. (1995) Simplification of emergency department discharge instructions improves patient comprehension. Annals of Emergency Medicine, 26, 443–446.

Medication adherence self-efficacy among low-literacy patients Kalichman, S.C. and Nachimson, D. (1999) Self-efficacy and disclosure of HIV positive serostatus to sex partners. Health Psychology, 18, 281–287. Kalichman, S.C., Catz, S. and Ramachandran, B. (1999) Barriers to HIV/AIDS treatment and treatment adherence among African-American adults with disadvantaged educations. Journal of the National Medical Association, 91, 439–446. Maibach, E. and Murphy, D. (1995) Self-efficacy in health promotion research and practice: conceptualization and measurement. Health Education Research, 10, 37–50. Mayeux, E.J., Murphy, P.W. and Arnold, C. (1996) Improving patient education for patients with low literacy. American Family Physician, 53, 205–211. McCormack, H., De Horne, D. and Sheater, S. (1988) Clinical applications of visual analogue scales: a critical review. Psychological Medicine, 18, 1007–1019.

Murphy, D.A., Multhauf, K.E. and Kalichman, S.C. (1995) Development and validation of a graded safer sex self-efficacy scale. Behavior Therapist, 18, 8–10. Schramm, U., Berger, G., Mueller, R., Kratzsch, T., Peters, J. and Froelich, L. (2002) Psychometric properties of Clock Drawing Test and MMSE or Short Performance Test (SKT) in dementia screening in a memory clinic population. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 17, 254–260. Ware J.E., Snow K.K., Kosinski, M. and Gandek, B. (1993) SF-36 Health Survey: Manual and Interpretation Guide. The Health Institute, New England Medical Center, Boston, MA. Wilkinson, G.S. (1993) The Wide Range Achievement Test, 3rd edn (WRAT-3). Wide Range, Wilmington, DE. Received on October 15, 2003; accepted on April 27, 2004

35

Suggest Documents