8. The current issue and full text archive of this journal is available at

The current issue and full text archive of this journal is available at www.emeraldinsight.com/0368-492X.htm K 40,7/8 A cybernetic model approach fo...
Author: Ann Powers
5 downloads 1 Views 130KB Size
The current issue and full text archive of this journal is available at www.emeraldinsight.com/0368-492X.htm

K 40,7/8

A cybernetic model approach for free jazz improvisations Jonas Braasch Graduate Program in Acoustics, School of Architecture, Rensselaer Polytechnic Institute, Troy, New York, USA

984 Abstract

Purpose – The purpose of this paper is to better understand communication between musicians in a free jazz improvisation in comparison to traditional jazz. Design/methodology/approach – A cybernetic informative feedback model was used to study communication between musicians for free jazz. The conceptual model consists of the ears as sensors, an auditory analysis stage to convert the acoustic signals into symbolic information (e.g. notated music), a cognitive processing stage (to make decisions and adapt the performance to what is being heard), and an effector (e.g. muscle movement to control an instrument). It was determined which musical features of the co-players have to be extracted to be able to respond adequately in a music improvisation, and how this knowledge can be used to build an automated music improvisation system for free jazz. Findings – The three major findings of this analysis were: in traditional jazz a soloist only needs to analyze a very limited set of music ensemble features, but in free jazz the performer has to observe each musician individually; unlike traditional jazz, free jazz is not a strict rule-based system. Consequently, the musicians need to develop their personal symbolic representation; which could be a machine-adequate music representation for an automated music improvisation system. The latter could be based on acoustic features that can be extracted robustly by a computer algorithm. Practical implications – Gained knowledge can be applied to build automated music improvisation systems for free jazz. Originality/value – The paper expands our knowledge to create intelligent music improvisation algorithms to algorithms that can improvise with a free jazz ensemble. Keywords Cybernetics, Automated music improvisation systems, Informative feedback models, Artificial creativity, Cognitive modeling, Free jazz, Auditory scene analysis, Music Paper type Conceptual paper

Introduction The purpose of the research reported here was to develop a model of jazz-ensemble improvisation practice to compare the communication structures for traditional jazz and free jazz. Cybernetics has been applied to understand several forms of music including traditional forms of jazz (Zaripov, 1969; Aufermann, 2005), but, to the author’s knowledge, not yet specifically to free jazz. The aim of the research reported is

Kybernetes Vol. 40 No. 7/8, 2011 pp. 984-994 q Emerald Group Publishing Limited 0368-492X DOI 10.1108/03684921111160214

This paper builds on knowledge that was gained in a project funded by the CreativeIT program of the National Science Foundation (No. 0757454). The concepts discussed in this paper will be part of an autonomous intelligent agent for free-music improvisation that the author is currently developing with colleagues Selmer Bringsjord, Pauline Oliveros, and Doug Van Nort with support from NSF (No. 1002851). The author would like to thank Ted Krueger and two anonymous reviewers for their helpful suggestions, and Kristen Murphy for proofreading the manuscript.

both to better understand the underlying mechanisms of free improvisation and the application of this knowledge to artificial-intelligent music systems. Unlike traditional jazz, free jazz, as indicated by its name, is not based on a rule system. The lack of such has led to a dramatic change in the way musicians communicate, which needs to be considered in the model. A major challenge for automated music improvisation systems has always been to operate with concrete sounds (e.g. analyzing in real time what is being played acoustically by musicians) instead of simply working on a symbolic, music theoretical level. The question of how the shift from traditional jazz to free jazz will affect this challenge is a central point of the research and addresses the second theme of the Cybernetics: Art, Design, Mathematics (C:ADM) 2010 International Conference: From Abstract to Actual (here: symbolic music representation vs actual sound). As will be discussed later, the solution asks for cross-over processes, the other theme of the C:ADM 2010 conference, and it needs to draw from knowledge in the fields of cybernetics/artificial intelligence, music theory, and psychoacoustics/machine listening. So far, the latter has only played a very limited role in automated music improvisation systems. The paper is organized as follows: . the introduction of the general cybernetic model structure that was used for this research, which builds on Wiener’s regulatory informative feedback model; . an overview of current automated improvisation systems for traditional jazz including a short description of the underlying musical concepts; . an outline of the music practices in free jazz compared to those in traditional jazz and how these practices affect the communication structure between musicians; . the introduction of a new model for an automated music improvisation system that addresses the specific needs of free improvisation including a comparison to algorithms that focus on traditional jazz; and . a concluding section. General cybernetic model structure based on informative feedback The approach that is described here starts with Wiener’s basic informative feedback model. Wiener (1961, p. 112 f.) explained his model based on the example of a person driving on an icy road. In this case, the effector is the muscular system that steers the car. The comparator, in Wiener’s case a simple subtractor, compares the desired trajectory of the car with the one the car actually took. The compensator then controls the effector in such a way that it brings the car onto the desired trajectory (e.g. by counter steering). For this study, Wiener’s model was extended (Figure 1), partly drawing from cybernetic models of Craik and Arbib, and also drawing from Pask’s ideas for his Musicolour system (Pickering, 2010, p. 313 ff). The “cognitive processes and memory” and “representation of world” stages of the model can be attributed to Craik (cited after Cordeschi, 2002, p. 138), and the model follows Arbib’s (1972, p. 129) definition of “goals as a ‘desired’ course of action from ‘higher’ centers”. In the case of jazz music, the effector is the musician’s muscular system that controls the musical instrument

A cybernetic model for free jazz 985

K 40,7/8

Goals

S

986

Auditory sensor (ears)

Auditory Analysis Pitch extraction on/offset detection timbre analysis beat analysis loudness estimation polyphonic analysis

Internal representation of world

S

S

S

Cognitive processes and memory S Comparator Compensator

Effector muscle control performing instrument

A

Figure 1. Informative feedback model to explain human information processing and action during a music performance

A A Acoustic signal flow S Symbolic information flow General signal/information flow

(e.g. a saxophone) to send out an acoustic signal. In a typical jazz performance, the signal is mixed with the sound of the other instruments before it is received by the musician’s auditory sensory organ. The sound mixture is analyzed by the auditory system and the extracted features are made available to the cognitive stages of the brain in the form of symbolic information. From a functional point of view, the system also includes at least one comparator (in this case likely to observe a multidimensional set of features), which compares the acoustic features to a given set of goals, and a compensator to align the actual performance with the desired goals. A very simple example could be an arranged piano solo, which, by the way, is not uncommon in Big Band music. Since the tuning of the piano is fixed and the notes to be played pre-determined, only the tempo has to be adjusted. If the piano is ahead of the remaining ensemble, the compensator would address the muscular system to slow down or, likewise, to play faster if the performer has fallen behind the band. Even for this very simple case, a major challenge is to extract all necessary information since all instruments, the piano and those of the remaining ensemble, overlap in time and frequency when their signals are perceived by the auditory system. Automated music improvisation systems for traditional jazz A brief overview on traditional jazz practices Matters become really interesting when the musician actually improvises a solo instead of playing one from sheet. Before this problem is addressed, it is useful to briefly introduce the main concepts and challenges in traditional jazz improvisations.

A more detailed introduction to jazz music theory can be found in Spitzer (2001) and several other publications dedicated to this topic. The term “traditional jazz” refers here to jazz styles that preceded the free jazz era, covering styles from swing to hardbop, but purposely excluding modal jazz, which already contained numerous elements that later became characteristic features of free jazz. In traditional jazz, the freedom of an improviser is more constrained than people outside this tradition might think. Typically, each solo follows the chord progression of the song that is played by the rhythm section. The latter typically consists of drums, bass, and one or more chordal instruments, predominantly piano or guitar. For traditional reasons, one chord progression cycle is called a “chorus”. The general repertoire of jazz tunes are called jazz standards, and most of these standards originated from Tin Pan Alley songs and pieces from Broadway musicals, in which the jazz musicians performed for a living. After the theme is played, the solo instruments take turns playing solos, and often the players of the rhythm section take their turns, too. In traditional jazz, the performer is free to play over as many choruses as he or she wants, but to end a solo before the end of the chord progression cycle is a taboo. The solo typically consists of a sequence of phrases that is chosen to match the chord progression and the intended dramaturgy. Since the two most common chord progressions in jazz are II-V and II-V-I (supertonic/dominant/tonic) combinations, professional jazz musicians train phrases based on these progressions. Extensive literature exists with collections of standard jazz phrases. Figure 2 shows the first eight bars of a notated saxophone solo over the 32-bar jazz standard, How High the Moon (Hamilton and Lewis, 1940) to provide a practical example. Charlie Parker’s Ornithology later used the same chord progression with a new bebop-style theme. Bars 3-6 consist of the typical II-V-I chord progression: Gm7 (notes: G, B[, D, F), C7 (C, E, G, B[), Fmaj7 (F, A, C, E), and Bars 7 and 8 of another II-V progression Fm7 (F, A[, C, E[) and B[7 (B[, D, F, A[). Notice how in the example the saxophone initially follows the notes of the individual chords closely with additional scale-related notes – which is typical for swing. From Bar 6 on, the phrases change to bebop style with a faster eighth-note pattern. Also noteworthy is the second half of Bar 7, where the saxophone plays note material outside the chord related scale to create a dissonant effect. Whether this is appropriate depends on the agreed upon rules; In the swing era this would have been, plainly put, incorrect play, but such techniques later became the characteristic style of players like Eric Dolphy, who could elegantly switch between the so-called inside and outside play.

1 Gmaj7

5 Fmaj7

2

3 Gm7

7 Fm7

6 3

A cybernetic model for free jazz 987

4 C7

8 B7

3

3 Outside

Figure 2. Example transcription of a saxophone solo over the jazz standard How High the Moon (first eight bars)

K 40,7/8

988

In order to play a correct solo following the rules of jazz theory, one could easily focus the attention to a very limited set of features to survive gracefully as shown in Figure 3 (It should be pointed out, though, that virtuoso jazz players are known to listen out and respond to many details initiated by the other players.). Basically, the soloist can deal with the rhythm section as a holistic entity, since all musicians follow the same chord progression. The tempo is quasi-stable, and the performance of the other soloist has to be observed only partially, to make sure not to cut into someone else’ turn. Once the soloist has been cleared to go ahead with his/her solo, he or she no longer needs to pay attention to the other soloists. Rule-based machine improvisation algorithms Numerous attempts have been made to design machine improvisation/composition algorithms to generate music material in the context of jazz and other styles (Cope, 1987; Friberg, 1991; Widmer, 1992; Jacob, 1996). In most cases, these algorithms use a symbolic language to code various music parameters. The wide-spread musical instrument digital interface (MIDI) format, for example, codes the fundamental frequencies of sounds into numbers. Here, the note C1 is the MIDI Number 24. Note numbers ascend in integers with the semitones. The temporal structure is also coded in numeral values related to a given rhythm and tempo structure. By utilizing such a symbolic code, improvisation or composition can become a mathematical problem. Typically, the program selects phrases from a database according to their fit to a given chord progression (e.g. avoiding tones that are outside the musical scales for these chords, as previously discussed in context of Figure 2), and current position in the bar structure (e.g. the program would not play a phrase ending in the beginning of a chord structure). Under such a paradigm, the quality of the machine performance can be evaluated fairly easily by testing whether any rules were Need to arrange order and duration of solos Soloist

Soloist

Needs to follow dramaturgy of soloists Rhythm section Piano/guitar

Figure 3. Schematic communication scheme for a traditional jazz performance

Bass Drums

• Needs to maintain beat with rhythm section • Needs to follow actual position in chord progression

violated or not. Of course, such an approach will not lead to a groundbreaking performance, but the results are often in line with the skills of a professional musician. A system can even operate in real time as long as it has access to the live music material on a symbolic level, for example MIDI data from an electronic keyboard. Lewis’ (2000) Voyager system and Pachet’s (2004) Continuator are working with MIDI data to interact with an individual performer. The system transforms and enhances the material of the human performer by generating new material from the received MIDI code, which can be derived from an acoustical sound source using an audio-to-MIDI converter. (Typically these systems fail if more than one musical instrument is included in the acoustic signal.) In the case of the Continuator, learning algorithms based on a Hidden Markov model help the system to copy the musical style of the human performer. Commercial systems that can improvise jazz are also available. The program “Band-in-a-Box” is an intelligent automatic accompaniment program that simulates a rhythm section for solo music entertainers. The system also simulates jazz solos for various instruments for a given chord progression and popular music style. The system can either generate a MIDI score that can be auralized using a MIDI synthesizer or create audio material by intelligently arranging pre-recorded jazz phrases. The restricted framework of the jazz tradition makes this quite possible, since the “listening” abilities of such a system can be limited to knowing the actual position within the form. Here the system needs to count along making sure that it keeps pace with the quasi-steady beat. Automated music improvisation systems for free jazz Free jazz music practices In contrast to traditional jazz, a formal set of rules does not exist in free jazz, although there has been a vivid tradition that has been carried on and expanded. Most of this tradition exists as tacit knowledge and is carried on in performance practice, orally and through musicological analyses. One example for tacit knowledge in free jazz is the taboo to perform traditional music material ( Jost, 1981), unless it is a brief reference in the context of other adequate free music material. For the application of the informative feedback model to free jazz, it is also important to understand how the tradition progressed over time deviating more and more from traditional jazz practice. A key moment for the development of free jazz was the introduction of modal jazz at the end of the 1950s, in which the chord progressions were replaced with fixed musical modes. In modal jazz the standard form of 12, 16 or 32 bars was initially kept, but one of the key moments of free jazz was that this structure was given up in the favor of a free (variable) duration of form. In the beginning, the music material was fairly traditional and could be analyzed based on traditional music notation (and thus can be easily captured using a symbolic music code like MIDI). However, later musicians started to use extended techniques that shifted their performance more and more from the traditional sound production techniques of the orchestral instruments used in jazz. Albert Mangelsdorff’s ability to perform multiphonics on the trombone is legendary, and so are the circular-breathed melodic streams of Evan Parker, who obtained the ability to perform arpeggio style continuous phrases with a variable overtone structure, containing both tonal and non-pitch-based elements. Peter Bro¨tzmann’s repertoire further expanded the techniques of non-pitched sounds. Among the younger generation of free jazz

A cybernetic model for free jazz 989

K 40,7/8

990

musicians are performers whose work focuses on complex musical textures outside the context of tonal music. Mazen Kerbaj (trumpet) and Christine Sehnaoui (saxophone) are among these players who have neglected the tonal heritage of their instruments in a unique way. Initially, free jazz musicians took turns to perform accompanied solos, but later they transformed it to a genre where the boundaries between solos and accompaniment were blurred. While in traditional jazz, a soloist has to listen to another soloist only to find a good slot for the solo, suddenly the performers had to pay attention all the time to the other soloists. In addition, the soloist could no longer rely on the predetermined role of the rhythm section, which was now allowed to change keys, tempo and/or style. The higher cognitive load that was necessary to observe all other participants in a session led to smaller ensembles, often duos. Larger ensembles like the Willem Breuker Kollektief remained as the exception. Machine improvisation algorithms for free (non-rule-based) music Figure 4 shows a model of communication during a free jazz session. The diagram, shown here for a group of three musicians, appears to be much simpler because of the lack of rules. In contrast to the previous model for traditional jazz (Figure 3), the distinction between rhythm section players and soloists is no longer made. While in traditional jazz the rhythm section can be dealt with as a holistic entity with homogeneous rhythm, tempo, and chord structure, now individual communication channels have to be built up between all musicians. Also, the feedback structure that each musician needs to enact to adequately respond to the other players is fundamentally different from traditional jazz, where the communication feedback loop (Figure 3) could simply cover a single communication stream from the ensemble (seen as a whole) to the soloist and back. In free music, a separate communication line has to be established between each possible pair of players, and consequently each performer has to divide his/her attention to observe all other players individually. Since the feedback from other musicians has to be detected with the ears, the multiple feedback-loop structure is not apparent in Figure 1. However, the need to extract the information individually for each musician from a complex sound field is what makes free music

Musician

Musician

Figure 4. Schematic communication scheme for a free jazz performance

Musician

Notes: The categorical distinction between soloists and rhythm section players no longer exists; each musician has to establish individual communication channels to all other musicians

improvisations a challenge. In addition, the performer always has to be prepared for the unexpected, especially since the tacit knowledge can be extended or modified within a session. With regard to the music parameter space, for traditional jazz it is sufficient to receive the pitches of the notes played, to determine the current chord structure and melody lines, and to capture the on- and offset times of these notes to align the performance in time with the rhythm section. Commercial audio-to-MIDI converters can perform this task reliably enough for this application if the general chord progression is known in advance. The analysis can even contain errors due to great information redundancy as long as the algorithm can follow the given chord progression. In the context of an automated system that can improvise free music, machine listening demands are much higher if the system is mimicking human performance (see the auditory analysis box in Figure 1). Now, we no longer have a pre-determined chord progression that serves as a general guideline. Even if we possessed a system that could extract the individual notes from a complex chord cluster – which, by the way, is difficult because of the complex overtone-structure of the individual notes – it is not guaranteed that the musical parameter space in a session is based on traditional music notes. To address this problem adequately, the intelligent system could be equipped with a complex model that simulates the auditory pathway. This type of model is able to extract features out of the acoustics signal in a similar way to the human brain (see Figure 1). The early stages of the auditory pathway (auditory periphery, early auditory nuclei perform the spectral decomposition, pitch estimation, on- and offset detection) are thought to be purely signal driven, whereas the performance of the higher stages (e.g. timbre recognition, recognition of musical structure) is strongly influenced by the individual background of a person, and these auditory features are categorized along learned patterns. The features extracted in the auditory analysis stage are coded as symbolic information and passed on to the cognitive processing stage. From the symbolic information it receives, it can construct an internal representation of the world (in this case, the representation of the jazz performance). As outlined in the previous section, the art of mapping acoustic signals onto symbolic information is well defined through jazz theory for traditional jazz. Thus, if the system does not know and follow the given rules, it will be easily detected by other musicians and the audience. In contrast, in free music there is no longer a standardized symbolic representation of what is being played. Instead, to a greater degree, the music is defined through its actual sound. Consequently, the musicians will need to derive their own symbolic representation to classify what they have heard and experienced, and they also need to define their own goals. For automated systems, the latter can be programmed using methods in second-order Cybernetics (Scott, 2004). With regard to symbolic music representation in humans, musicians typically draw from their own musical background, and significant differences can be found for musicians who primarily received classical music training compared to those who concentrated on jazz or chose to work with sound textures rather than pitch and harmony. These differences extend to artists who grew up in a non-Western music tradition. For example, if a typically trained musician hears a musical scale, he or she associates it with a scale that exists in his or her musical culture. This association works as long as the individual pitches of each note fall within a certain tolerance. Consequently, two people from two different

A cybernetic model for free jazz 991

K 40,7/8

992

Table I. Listening strengths and weaknesses of machines compared to humans

cultural backgrounds could label the same scale differently, and thus operate in different symbolic worlds judging the same acoustic events. In free music, interactions between musicians of various cultural backgrounds are often anticipated, hoping that these types of collaborations will lead to new forms of music and preclude musicians falling into stereotypic patterns. The communication, however, will only work if the structures of the different musical systems have enough overlap so the musicians can decipher a sufficient amount of features of the other performing musicians into their own system. Furthermore, as performers, we have only indirect access to the listening ability of the co-musicians through observing what they play, and in case something was not “perceived” correctly by others, we cannot measure their resulting response (musical action) along rules in free music, because they do not exist. Furthermore, for cross-cultural music ensembles, examples exist where communication problems resulted from operating in different music systems. The late Yulius Golombeck once said that when he was performing with the world music band “Embryo”, Charlie Mariano, and the Karnataka College of Percussion, there were certain complex Indian rhythms played by the Karnataka College of Percussion that the Western trained musicians could not participate in because the rhythmical structure was too complicated to understand, despite the fact that all musicians had a tremendous experience with non-Western music (personal communication, 1995). While the complex communication structure in free music poses a real challenge for automated music systems, the lack of a standardized symbolic representation can be played to a system’s advantage. Instead of mimicking the auditory system to extract the musical features (Figure 1), an alternative approach could be a robot-adequate design. The design could consider that as of today some parameters (e.g. complex chords) are impossible to extract in parallel for multiple musicians, especially in the presence of room reverberation. Instead, a music culture for machines could be developed that emphasizes the strengths of machines and circumvents their shortcomings. The latter are summarized in Table I. A directed focus on machine-adequate listening algorithms would also encourage the design of machines to have their own identity, instead of focusing on making them indistinguishable from humans on passing the Turing test (e.g. compare Boden, 2010). Man/machine communication could then be treated like a cross-cultural performance, where sufficient overlap between the various cultures is expected to allow a meaningful Listening strengths Listening weakness Absolute sense of pitch, timbre, timing and most Have difficulty to extract information from other information multiple source and reverberant environments Difficulty to reconstruct missing information Difficulty to perceptually correct imperfections of other players Cognition strength Cognition weakness Absolute memory Not good in abstraction –not able to develop new concepts Good at combinatorics Difficulties in judging the quality of a performance Action strength Action weakness Redefines the ideal of virtuosity, can play Difficulty to perform material with great musical anything at any tempo without imperfections expression

communication. In such collaborations, the highlight would not be to replace humans with machines, but to build systems that inspire human performers in a unique and creative way. A good example of machine inspired human music performance in another context was the introduction of the drum machine, which encouraged a new generation of drummers around Dave Weckl in the 1980s to perform their instruments more accurately, almost in a machine-like style.

A cybernetic model for free jazz 993

Conclusion The purpose of this study was to develop a cybernetic model to understand different forms of jazz by bridging the fields of cybernetics, music theory, psychoacoustics and machine listening. Wiener’s regulatory informative feedback model was chosen as a starting point. It was further developed to serve as a model for jazz improvisation, using elements of other cybernetic models by Arbib and Craik. The model was then used to investigate the communication structure between jazz musicians with a focus on which musical features have to be extracted by a soloist in order to adapt his/her own performance to what is being played by the ensemble. This was accomplished using an informative feedback process. The analysis revealed that the communication structure in free jazz is fundamentally different from traditional jazz: . The distinguished roles between lead improviser and backing ensemble no longer exist and the improviser can no longer concentrate on a single musical stream. In an informative feedback model this results in a parallel feedback-loop system (one for each musician). Owing to the lack of explicit rules, it can be less obvious if the performer misinterprets what is being played by others. . Since free jazz is not rule based, but defined through actual sounds, a general theory on how to convert the acoustic events into symbolic code no longer exists. . Consequently, automated music improvisation systems do not necessarily have to follow human practices but can instead use a symbolic code that focuses on features that can be extracted robustly by computers. Here, it is important, though, that there is sufficient overlap between both symbolic codes, so human performers will find that the machine responds in a useful and systematic manner. References Arbib, M.A. (1972), The Metaphorical Brain: An Introduction to Cybernetics as Artificial Intelligence and Brain Theory, Wiley, New York, NY. Aufermann, K. (2005), “Feedback and music: you provide the noise, the order comes by itself”, Kybernetes, Vol. 34 Nos 3/4, p. 490. Boden, M.A. (2010), “The turing test and artistic creativity”, Kybernetes, Vol. 39, p. 409. Cope, D. (1987), “An expert system for computer-assisted composition”, Computer Music Journal, Vol. 11 No. 4, pp. 30-46. Cordeschi, R. (2002), The Discovery of the Artificial: Behavior, Mind, and Machines Before and Beyond Cybernetics, Kluwer, Amsterdam. Friberg, A. (1991), “Generative rules for music performance: a formal description of a rule system”, Computer Music Journal, Vol. 15 No. 2, pp. 56-71. Hamilton, N. and Lewis, M. (1940), How High the Moon, available at: http://en.wikipedia.org/ wiki/How_High_the_Moon

K 40,7/8

994

Jacob, B. (1996), “Algorithmic composition as a model of creativity”, Organised Sound, Vol. 1 No. 3, pp. 157-65. Jost, E. (1981), Free Jazz, DaCapo, New York, NY. Lewis, G.E. (2000), “Too many notes: computers, complexity and culture in voyager”, Leonardo Music Journal, Vol. 10, pp. 33-9. Pachet, F. (2004), “Beyond the cybernetic jam fantasy: the continuator”, IEEE Computer Graphics and Applications, Vol. 24 No. 1, pp. 31-5. Pickering, A. (2010), The Cybernetic Brain – Sketches of Another Future, University of Chicago Press, London. Scott, B. (2004), “Second-order cybernetics: an historical introduction”, Kybernetes, Vol. 33 Nos 9/10, pp. 1365-78. Spitzer, P. (2001), Jazz Theory Handbook, Mel Bay Publications, Pacific, MO. Widmer, G. (1992), “Qualitative perception modeling and intelligent musical learning”, Computer Music Journal, Vol. 16 No. 2, pp. 51-68. Wiener, N. (1961), Cybernetics or Control and Communication in the Animal and the Machine, 2nd ed., The MIT Press, New York, NY. Zaripov, R.K. (1969), “Cybernetics and music”, Perspectives of New Music, Vol. 7 No. 2, pp. 115-54. Further reading Widmer, G. (1994), “The synergy of music theory and AI: learning multi-level expressive interpretation”, Technical Report OEFAI-94-06, Austrian Research Institute for Artificial Intelligence, Vienna. About the author Jonas Braasch is an Acoustician, Musicologist, and Sound Artist who teaches courses in Acoustics & Music at the School of Architecture at Rensselaer Polytechnic Institute. He obtained a Master’s degree from Dortmund University (Germany, 1998) in Physics and two PhD degrees from Ruhr-University Bochum, Germany (2001, 2004) in Electrical Engineering/Information Science and Musicology. His research interests include binaural hearing, multi-channel audio technology, telematic music systems, perceptual audio/visual integration, intelligent systems, and musical acoustics. For his work, he has received funding from NSF, NSERC (Canada), DFG (German Science Foundation), and NYSCA. As a soprano saxophonist and sound artist, he has on-going collaborations with Curtis Bahn, Chris Chafe, Michael Century, Mark Dresser, Pauline Oliveros, and Doug van Nort – among others. Jonas Braasch is a Board Member of the Deep Listening Institute (Kingston, NY), EMPAC affiliated faculty, and holds an Adjunct Professor Appointment with the Schulich School of Music at McGill University. Jonas Braasch can be contacted at: [email protected]

To purchase reprints of this article please e-mail: [email protected] Or visit our web site for further details: www.emeraldinsight.com/reprints

Suggest Documents